Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Scientific context  





2 Legal  





3 See also  





4 References  














Anecdotal evidence






Afrikaans
العربية
Български
Català
Čeština
Dansk
Deutsch
Español
Français

Bahasa Indonesia
עברית

Bahasa Melayu
Nederlands

Norsk bokmål
Polski
Português
Русский
Suomi
Svenska

Українська


 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 

(Redirected from Anecdata)

Anecdotal evidence is evidence based only on personal observation, collected in a casual or non-systematic manner.

"Anecdotal" can refer to:

1. Relaying personal experiences[1] or sense data[2], also called testimony, or a testimonial[3].

2. Relaying the words or experiences of another[4] named person[5], sometimes called hearsay[6].

3. Relaying an account from an fictional source, or story[7] with no attribution, also called an apocryphal[8] saying, an Old Wives' Tale, a Myth, or folklore.

Because the term connotes three very different kinds of evidence, discussion of the term can result in accidental or intentional equivocation , where people are talking about different meanings of the term without realizing it. Since an anecdote may be real or fictional[5], it is often difficult to talk about this form of evidence as a category without explaining exactly what type of anecdotal evidence is being referenced.

Anecdotal Evidence usually is not subject to rules of legal, historical, academic, or intellectual rigor, meaning that there are little or no safeguards against fabrication or inaccuracy. This does not mean that all anecdotal evidence is false, it just means that the methodology of the scholarly method, the scientific method, or legal requirements of testimony have not been required of the evidence.

Because the term connotes three very different kinds of evidence, discussion of the term can result in accidental or intentional equivocation , where people are talking about different meanings of the term without realizing it. Since an anecdote may be real or fictional[9], it is often difficult to talk about this form of evidence as a category without explaining exactly what type of anecdotal evidence is being referenced.

Anecdotal Evidence usually is not subject to rules of legal, historical, academic, or intellectual rigor, meaning that there are little or no safeguards against fabrication or inaccuracy. This does not mean that all anecdotal evidence is false, it just means that the methodology of scholarly method or the scientific method, or legal requirements of testimony have not been required of the evidence.

When used in advertising or promotion of a product, service, or idea, anecdotal reports are often called a testimonial, which are highly regulated[10] in some jurisdictions.

Anecdotal evidence may be considered within the scope of scientific method as some anecdotal evidence can be both empirical and verifiable, e.g. in the use of case studies in medicine. Other anecdotal evidence, however, does not qualify as scientific evidence, because its nature prevents it from being investigated by the scientific method, for instance, in that of folklore or in the case of intentionally fictional anecdotes. Where only one or a few anecdotes are presented, there is a chance that they may be unreliable due to cherry-picked or otherwise non-representative samples of typical cases.[11][12] Similarly, psychologists have found that due to cognitive bias people are more likely to remember notable or unusual examples rather than typical examples.[13] Thus, even when accurate, anecdotal evidence is not necessarily representative of a typical experience. Accurate determination of whether an anecdote is typical requires statistical evidence.[14] Misuse of anecdotal evidence in the form of argument from anecdote is an informal fallacy[15] and is sometimes referred to as the "person who" fallacy ("I know a person who..."; "I know of a case where..." etc.) which places undue weight on experiences of close peers which may not be typical.

Scientific context[edit]

In science, definitions of anecdotal evidence include:

Anecdotal evidence can have varying degrees of formality. For instance, in medicine, published anecdotal evidence by a trained observer (a doctor) is called a case report, and is subjected to formal peer review.[20] Although such evidence is not seen as conclusive, researchers may sometimes regard it as an invitation to more rigorous scientific study of the phenomenon in question.[21] For instance, one study found that 35 of 47 anecdotal reports of drug side-effects were later sustained as "clearly correct."[22]

Anecdotal evidence is considered the least certain type of scientific information.[23] Researchers may use anecdotal evidence for suggesting new hypotheses, but never as validating evidence.[24][25]

If an anecdote illustrates a desired conclusion rather than a logical conclusion, it is considered a faultyorhasty generalization.[26]

In any case where some factor affects the probability of an outcome, rather than uniquely determining it, selected individual cases to prove nothing; e.g. "my grandfather smoked two packs a day until he died at 90" and "my sister never smoked but died of lung cancer". Anecdotes often refer to the exception, rather than the rule: "Anecdotes are useless precisely because they may point to idiosyncratic responses."[27]

In medicine, anecdotal evidence is also subject to placebo effects.[28]

Legal[edit]

In the Legal sphere, anecdotal evidence, if it passes certain legal requirements and is admitted as testimony, is a common form of evidence used in a court of law. Often this form of anecdotal evidence is the only evidence presented at trial.[29] Scientific evidence in a court of law is called physical evidence, but this is much rarer. Anecdotal evidence, with a few safeguards, represents the bulk of evidence in court.

The legal rigors applied to testimony for it to be evidence is under oath, and that the person is only testifying to their own words and actions, and that someone intentionally lying under oath is subject to perjury. However, these rigors do not make testimony in a court of law equal to scientific evidence as there are far less legal rigors. Testimony about another person's experiences or words is called hearsay. However, any hearsay that is not objected to or thrown out by a judge is considered evidence for a jury. This means that trials contain quite a bit of anecdotal evidence, which is considered as relavant evidence by a jury. Eyewitness testimony (which is a form of anecdotal evidence) is considered the most compelling form of evidence by a jury[30].


See also[edit]

  • Argument from ignorance – Informal fallacy
  • Confirmation bias – Bias confirming existing attitudes
  • Correlation does not imply causation – Refutation of a logical fallacy
  • Empirical evidence – Knowledge acquired by means of the senses
  • Eyewitness testimony – Account a witness gives in the courtroom of what they observed
  • Fallacy – Argument that uses faulty reasoning
  • Faulty generalization – Conclusion made on the basis of one or few instances of a phenomenon
  • Hasty generalization – Conclusion made on the basis of one or few instances of a phenomenon
  • List of fallacies – List of faulty argument types
  • Lived experience – Phenomenological concept
  • Post hoc ergo propter hoc – Fallacy of assumption of causality based on sequence of events
  • Presumption of guilt – Presumption that a person is guilty of a crime
  • Scientific method – Interplay between observation, experiment, and theory in science
  • References[edit]

    1. ^ Michal, Audrey (2021). "When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making". Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 6. doi:10.1186/s41235-021-00293-2. PMID 33825055.
  • ^ "Mirriam Webster".
  • ^ "MIrriam Webster".
  • ^ Michal, Audrey. [When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making "When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making"]. PMID 33825055. {{cite web}}: Check |url= value (help)
  • ^ "Mirriam Webster".
  • ^ "Mirriam Webster".
  • ^ "Mirriam Webster".
  • ^ "Definition of APOCRYPHAL". Mirriam Webster.
  • ^ "Anecdote Wikipedia".
  • ^ "Guides Concerning the Use of Endorsements and Testimonials in Advertising" (PDF). FTC.gov.
  • ^ Weiten, Wayne (2010). Psychology: Themes and Variations. Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. p. 75. ISBN 9780495601975.
  • ^ Goodwin, C. James (2009). Research in Psychology: Methods and Design. John Wiley & Sons. p. 25. ISBN 9780470522783.
  • ^ Gibson, Rhonda; Zillman, Dolf (1994). "Exaggerated Versus Representative Exemplification in News Reports: Perception of Issues and Personal Consequences". Communication Research. 21 (5): 603–624. doi:10.1177/009365094021005003. S2CID 145050644.
  • ^ Schwarcz, Joe; Barrett, Stephen. "Some Notes on the Nature of Science". Archived from the original on 20 September 2012. Retrieved 16 June 2022.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  • ^ "Fallacies | Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy". www.iep.utm.edu. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  • ^ "anecdotal". YourDictionary.com. Retrieved 17 June 2019.
  • ^ "Nechako White Sturgeon Recovery Initiative - Glossary - NWSRI". www.nechakowhitesturgeon.org. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  • ^ "Anecdotal evidence - Smart Health Choices - NCBI Bookshelf". www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  • ^ "No Love for Anecdotal Evidence". NeuroLogica Blog. 2007-03-08. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  • ^ Jenicek, M. (1999). Clinical Case Reporting in Evidence-Based Medicine. Oxford: Butterworth–Heinemann. p. 117. ISBN 0-7506-4592-X.
  • ^ Vandenbroucke, J. P. (2001). "In Defense of Case Reports and Case Series". Annals of Internal Medicine. 134 (4): 330–334. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-134-4-200102200-00017. PMID 11182844. S2CID 867759.
  • ^ Venning, G. R. (1982). "Validity of anecdotal reports of suspected adverse drug reactions: the problem of false alarms". Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 284 (6311): 249–52. doi:10.1136/bmj.284.6311.249. PMC 1495801. PMID 0006799125.
  • ^ Riffenburgh, R. H. (1999). Statistics in Medicine. Boston: Academic Press. pp. 196. ISBN 0-12-588560-1.
  • ^ Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Lynn, Steven Jay; Lohr, Jeffrey M. (2014). "Initial Thoughts, Reflections, and Considerations". Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology (2 ed.). New York: Guilford Publications. p. 9. ISBN 9781462517510. Testimonial and anecdotal evidence can be quite useful in the early stages of scientific investigation. Nevertheless, such evidence is almost always much more helpful in the context of discovery (i.e., hypothesis generation) than in the context of justification (i.e., hypothesis testing [...]).
  • ^ Mebius, A. (2022). "Against 'instantaneous' expertise". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 17 (11): 11. doi:10.1186/s13010-022-00123-3. PMC 9490894. PMID 36127693. S2CID 252384889.
  • ^ Thompson B. Fallacies. Archived April 20, 2006, at the Wayback Machine
  • ^ Sicherer, Scott H. (1999). "Food allergy: When and how to perform oral food challenges". Pediatric Allergy and Immunology. 10 (4): 226–234. doi:10.1034/j.1399-3038.1999.00040.x. PMID 10678717. S2CID 1484234.
  • ^ "Evaluating Treatment Products". MedicineNet.
  • ^ "The Judicial Learning Center". 10 August 2012.
  • ^ "Benton, Ross, Bradshaw, Thomas, & Bradshaw, 2006".

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Anecdotal_evidence&oldid=1232312772"

    Categories: 
    Informal fallacies
    Philosophy of science
    Skepticism
    Evidence
    Testimony
    Inductive fallacies
    Pseudoscience
    Diversionary tactics
    Misuse of statistics
    Anecdotes
    Hidden categories: 
    CS1 errors: URL
    CS1 maint: unfit URL
    Webarchive template wayback links
    Articles with short description
    Short description is different from Wikidata
    Pages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets via Module:Annotated link
     



    This page was last edited on 3 July 2024, at 02:56 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki