Home  

Random  

Nearby  



Log in  



Settings  



Donate  



About Wikipedia  

Disclaimers  



Wikipedia





Observation





Article  

Talk  



Language  

Watch  

Edit  





Observation in the natural sciences[1] is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving [2] and the acquisition of information from a primary source. In living beings, observation employs the senses. In science, observation can also involve the perception and recording of data via the use of scientific instruments. The term may also refer to any data collected during the scientific activity. Observations can be qualitative, that is, the absence or presence of a property is noted and the observed phenomenon described, or quantitative if a numerical value is attached to the observed phenomenonbycountingormeasuring.

Observing the air traffic in Rõuge, Estonia

Science

edit

The scientific method requires observations of natural phenomena to formulate and test hypotheses.[3] It consists of the following steps:[4][5]

  1. Ask a question about a natural phenomenon
  2. Make observations of the phenomenon
  3. Formulate a hypothesis that tentatively answers the question
  4. Predict logical, observable consequences of the hypothesis that have not yet been investigated
  5. Test the hypothesis' predictions by an experiment, observational study, field study, or simulation
  6. Draw a conclusion from data gathered in the experiment, or revise the hypothesis or form a new one and repeat the process
  7. Write a descriptive method of observation and the results or conclusions reached
  8. Have peers with experience researching the same phenomenon evaluate the results

Observations play a role in the second and fifth steps of the scientific method. However, the need for reproducibility requires that observations by different observers can be comparable. Human sense impressions are subjective and qualitative, making them difficult to record or compare. The use of measurement was developed to allow recording and comparison of observations made at different times and places, by different people. The measurement consists of using observation to compare the phenomenon being observed to a standard unit. The standard unit can be an artifact, process, or definition which can be duplicated or shared by all observers. In measurement, the number of standard units which is equal to the observation is counted. Measurement reduces an observation to a number that can be recorded, and two observations which result in the same number are equal within the resolution of the process.

Human senses are limited and subject to errors in perception, such as optical illusions. Scientific instruments were developed to aid human abilities of observation, such as weighing scales, clocks, telescopes, microscopes, thermometers, cameras, and tape recorders, and also translate into perceptible form events that are unobservable by the senses, such as indicator dyes, voltmeters, spectrometers, infrared cameras, oscilloscopes, interferometers, Geiger counters, and radio receivers.

One problem encountered throughout scientific fields is that the observation may affect the process being observed, resulting in a different outcome than if the process was unobserved. This is called the observer effect. For example, it is not normally possible to check the air pressure in an automobile tire without letting out some of the air, thereby changing the pressure. However, in most fields of science, it is possible to reduce the effects of observation to insignificance by using better instruments.

Considered as a physical process itself, all forms of observation (human or instrumental) involve amplification and are thus thermodynamically irreversible processes, increasing entropy.

Paradoxes

edit

In some specific fields of science, the results of observation differ depending on factors that are not important in everyday observation. These are usually illustrated with apparent "paradoxes" in which an event appears different when observed from two different points of view, seeming to violate "common sense".

Biases

edit

The human senses do not function like a video camcorder, impartially recording all observations.[6] Human perception occurs by a complex, unconscious process of abstraction, in which certain details of the incoming sense data are noticed and remembered, and the rest is forgotten. What is kept and what is thrown away depends on an internal model or representation of the world, called by psychologists a schema, that is built up over our entire lives. The data is fitted into this schema. Later when events are remembered, memory gaps may even be filled by "plausible" data the mind makes up to fit the model; this is called reconstructive memory. How much attention the various perceived data are given depends on an internal value system, which judges how important it is to the individual. Thus two people can view the same event and come away with entirely different perceptions of it, even disagreeing about simple facts. This is why eyewitness testimony is notoriously unreliable.[citation needed]

Several of the more important ways observations can be affected by human psychology are given below.

Confirmation bias

edit

Human observations are biased toward confirming the observer's conscious and unconscious expectations and view of the world; we "see what we expect to see".[7] In psychology, this is called confirmation bias.[7] Since the object of scientific research is the discovery of new phenomena, this bias can and has caused new discoveries to be overlooked; one example is the discovery of x-rays. It can also result in erroneous scientific support for widely held cultural myths, on the other hand, as in the scientific racism that supported ideas of racial superiority in the early 20th century.[8] Correct scientific technique emphasizes careful recording of observations, separating experimental observations from the conclusions drawn from them, and techniques such as blindordouble blind experiments, to minimize observational bias.

Processing bias

edit

Modern scientific instruments can extensively process "observations" before they are presented to the human senses, and particularly with computerized instruments, there is sometimes a question as to where in the data processing chain "observing" ends and "drawing conclusions" begins. This has recently become an issue with digitally enhanced images published as experimental data in papersinscientific journals. The images are enhanced to bring out features that the researcher wants to emphasize, but this also has the effect of supporting the researcher's conclusions. This is a form of bias that is difficult to quantify. Some scientific journals have begun to set detailed standards for what types of image processing are allowed in research results. Computerized instruments often keep a copy of the "raw data" from sensors before processing, which is the ultimate defense against processing bias, and similarly, scientific standards require preservation of the original unenhanced "raw" versions of images used as research data.[citation needed]

Philosophy

edit

InCosmology the origins of observation are related with the origins of evolutions in our cosmos.[9]

Process philosophy is the changing relationships of our senses, minds and experiencestoourselves.[10]

"Observe always that everything is the result of a change, and get used to thinking that there is nothing Nature loves so well as to change existing forms and to make new ones like them."

— Meditations. iv. 36. – Marcus Aurelius

See also

edit
  • Extrospection
  • Introspection
  • List of cognitive biases
  • Metaphysics of presence
  • Naturalistic observation
  • Observation unit
  • Observational astronomy
  • Observational error
  • Observational learning
  • Observational study
  • Observable quantity
  • Observations and Measurements
  • Observatory
  • Observer effect
  • Noumenon
  • Present
  • Self
  • Theory ladenness
  • Uncertainty principle
  • Unobservable
  • References

    edit
    1. ^ "Philosophy of Cosmology". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. 2017.
  • ^ "Meanings and Definitions of Words at". Dictionary.com. Retrieved 2022-05-13.
  • ^ Kosso, Peter (2011). A Summary of Scientific Method. Springer. p. 9. ISBN 978-9400716131.
  • ^ Mendez, Carl Cedrick L.; Heller, H. Craig; Berenbaum, May (2009). Life: The Science of Biology, 9th Ed. US: Macmillan. pp. 13–14. ISBN 978-1429219624.
  • ^ Shipman, James; Wilson, Jerry D.; Todd, Aaron (2009). Introduction to Physical Science, 12th Ed. Cengage Learning. p. 4. ISBN 978-0538731874.
  • ^ Shaw, Julia (Aug 12, 2016). "Not all memories happened: What experts wish you knew about false memories". Scientific American. Nature America, Inc. Retrieved August 13, 2016.
  • ^ a b Shermer, Michael (2002). Why People Believe Weird Things: Pseudoscience, Superstition, and Other Confusions of Our Time. MacMillan. pp. 299–302. ISBN 1429996765.
  • ^ Gardner, Martin (1957). Fads and Fallacies in the Name of Science. Dover Publications, Inc. pp. 152–163. ISBN 9780486131627.
  • ^ "Philosophy of Cosmology". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. 2017.
  • ^ "Process Philosophy". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. 2022.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Observation&oldid=1217270575"
     



    Last edited on 4 April 2024, at 20:44  





    Languages

     


    العربية
    Asturianu
    Azərbaycanca
    Български
    Bosanski
    Català
    Čeština
    ChiShona
    Dansk
    Eesti
    Ελληνικά
    Español
    Esperanto
    Euskara
    فارسی
    Français
    Galego

    Հայերեն
    ि
    Hrvatski
    Bahasa Indonesia
    Italiano
    עברית

    Latina
    Latviešu

    Bahasa Melayu
    Монгол
    Nederlands


    Norsk bokmål
    Norsk nynorsk
    Occitan

    پښتو
    Polski
    Português
    Română
    Русский
    Simple English
    سنڌي
    Slovenčina
    کوردی
    Српски / srpski
    Srpskohrvatski / српскохрватски
    Suomi
    Svenska
    Tagalog
    ி

    Türkçe
    Українська
    Tiếng Vit
    Winaray



     

    Wikipedia


    This page was last edited on 4 April 2024, at 20:44 (UTC).

    Content is available under CC BY-SA 4.0 unless otherwise noted.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Terms of Use

    Desktop