Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Main paradigms  





2 Component characteristics  



2.1  Development  





2.2  Morphology  





2.3  Topography  







3 Functional sensitivity  



3.1  Attention and expectancy  





3.2  Probability  





3.3  Intensity  





3.4  Interstimulus interval  







4 O-wave and E-wave  





5 Localization  





6 Theory  





7 See also  





8 References  














Contingent negative variation






Español
 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


The contingent negative variation (CNV) is a negative slow surface potential, as measured by electroencephalography (EEG), that occurs during the period between a warning stimulus or signal and an imperative ("go") stimulus. [1] The CNV was one of the first event-related potential (ERP) components to be described. The CNV component was first described by W. Grey Walter and colleagues in an article published in Nature in 1964.[2] The importance of this finding was that it was one of the first studies which showed that consistent patterns of the amplitude of electric responses could be obtained from the large background noise which occurs in EEG recordings and that this activity could be related to a cognitive process such as expectancy.

Main paradigms[edit]

In their study, Grey Walter et al. (1964) [2] presented a "warning stimulus" (e.g., a single click or flash of light) to a human subject. The warning stimulus was randomly followed (or not followed) by an "imperative stimulus" (repetitive clicks or flashes) 0.5-1 second later. In some situations, the subject simply experienced warning and imperative stimuli passively; in others, the subject could prevent the imperative stimulus, if it occurred, by a behavioral response, e.g., pressing a button, but the button worked only if it was pressed after the imperative stimulus had occurred (i.e., premature button presses were ineffective). The process of warning stimulus optionally followed by imperative stimulus recurred at variable intervals between 3–10 sec.

The experimenters found that EEG responses to the warning stimulus seemed to have three phases: a brief positive component, a brief negative component, and a sustained negative component. The brief components varied with sensory modality (e.g., visual vs auditory), while the sustained component varied with the time gap between the warning and imperative stimuli (and whether the imperative stimulus occurred) and the subject's attention/vigilance. Walter et al labeled the sustained component the "contingent negative variation" because the variation of the negative wave was contingent on the statistical relationship between the warning and imperative stimuli. They observed that:

In 1990 Bozinovska et al devised a CNV-based brain-computer interface to control a computer buzzer.[3][4] In 2009, Bozinovski and Bozinovska designed a CNV-based brain-computer interface experiment, where the CNV controlled a physical object, a robot.[5]

Component characteristics[edit]

Walter et al. (1964) showed that a single click elicits a brief positive peak and a brief negative peak. Repetitive flashes elicit brief positive and negative peaks. If these stimuli are separated by 1 sec the same individual patterns result. After around 50 presentations, these peaks are indistinguishable from noise. On the other hand, when a single click is followed by the repetitive flashes which are terminated by a button press, there is a large gradual negative peak which ends sharply with the button press. This is the contingent negative variation. Another classical study was described by Joseph Tecce in the Psychological Bulletin in 1972.[6] In this review, Tecce summarizes the development, morphology, and locus of appearance of the CNV.

Development[edit]

Studies have shown that the CNV appears after about 30 trials of paired stimuli, although this number can be reduced when the subject understands the task in advance. Light flashes, clicks, and tones have all been used to elicit the CNV. A response to the imperative stimulus is necessary to elicit a clear CNV. This response could be a physical or mental response.[6] The CNV is elicited when two, linked stimuli are presented. When the imperative stimulus is removed unexpectedly, the CNV attenuates until it is completely suppressed after about 20–50 trials. The CNV is immediately restored if paired with the imperative stimulus again.

Morphology[edit]

The negative CNV peak rises around 260–470 ms after the warning stimulus. It will rise quickly if the subject is uncertain about when the imperative stimulus will be, and it will rise gradually if the subject is confident about when the imperative stimulus will be. The maximum amplitude is usually around 20 microvolts.[6]

Topography[edit]

The CNV appears most prominently at the vertex and is bilaterally symmetrical.[6]

Functional sensitivity[edit]

There is much research which describes what stimulus characteristics can affect characteristics of the CNV. For example, intensity, modality, duration, stimulus rate, probability, stimulus relevance, and pitch discrimination can affect the CNV component.[7]

Attention and expectancy[edit]

Attention also affects the amplitude of the CNV. The following examples from various task conditions and studies show that the CNV is changed when the experimental protocol changes the attention needed to perform the tasks.[2][6] First, when subjects were told that the imperative stimulus would be removed, the CNV was reduced. Second, in one condition subjects were allowed to choose whether they were going to press the button or not. In trials where the subject chose not to respond, there was no CNV. Third, when the subject was specifically told that there would not be repetitive flashes, no CNV was elicited. Fourth, another condition showed that a CNV was elicited in subjects who were told to estimate when the repetitive flashes would come even when no flashes were presented. Fifth, when subjects were asked to pay attention and respond quickly, CNV amplitude was increased. The results of these conditions suggest that the CNV is related to attention and expectancy.

Probability[edit]

When the probability of repetitive flashes is random and the repetitive flashes are removed in about 50% of the trials, the amplitude of the CNV is about half as that of normal.

Intensity[edit]

Some researchers have shown that the intensity of the stimulus may affect the CNV amplitude. It seems that the CNV component has a higher amplitude for stimuli that have low-intensity, i.e. is difficult to see or hear, as opposed to stimuli that have high-intensity. This could be because the subject must pay more attention to perceive the low-intensity stimulus. If the detection of the imperative task becomes too difficult, then the CNV amplitude is reduced. In other words, attention to the imperative stimulus is important for the development of the CNV and increased task difficulties distract the attention.

In related studies, researchers have also shown that the larger the motoric response needed, the larger the CNV. Studies with subjects that have a lack of sleep tend to show a reduced CNV. This provides further evidence that lack of attention might decrease the CNV amplitude.[6]

Interstimulus interval[edit]

The amplitude of the CNV changes when one changes the foreperiod, or interstimulus interval (ISI). The most frequent ISI used is between 1.0–1.5 seconds. Trials with an ISI between 0.5–1.5 elicit a robust CNV wave. When the ISI is reduced to 0.125 or 0.25 seconds, the CNV becomes suppressed. On the other hand, trials with an ISI of 4.8 seconds show reduced CNV amplitude.

O-wave and E-wave[edit]

Most researchers agree that the CNV component has been associated with information processing and response preparation. The main controversy is whether the CNV is composed of more than one component. After discovery of the CNV, researchers were able to distinguish between two main components of the CNV. Loveless and Sanford (1975) and Weerts and Lang (1973) increased the interstimulus interval to greater than 3 seconds and showed that two components can be visually distinguished from the CNV. The first wave followed the warning stimulus and was called the O wave, or orienting wave.[8][9] This wave showed enhanced amplitude in the frontal regions. The second wave preceded the imperative stimulus and was called the E wave, or expectancy wave. A study conducted by Gaillard (1976) provided further evidence that the O wave was frontally distributed and was more strongly affected by auditory stimuli rather than visual stimuli.[10]

A related, important issue has been the question of whether all or part of the CNV corresponds to the readiness potential. The readiness potential is the neural preparation for motoric responses. Both components have a similar scalp distribution with a negative amplitude and are associated with a motor response. In fact, many researchers claimed that the terminal CNV, or E wave, was in fact the readiness potential, or Bereitschaftspotential. This was the general consensus until other work provided evidence that the CNV can be distinguished from the RP.[7][11] First, the RP is usually lateralized to the contralateral side of the motoric response, while the CNV is usually bilateral. Second, the CNV can occur even when a motor response is not required. Third, a RP occurs without any external stimuli. This shows that the RP occurs for motor responses while the CNV occurs when two stimuli are contingent with each other.[6]

Localization[edit]

Another important topic in studying the CNV component is localizing the general source of the CNV. For example, Hultin, Rossini, Romani, Högstedt, Tecchio, and Pizzella (1996) used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to determine the location of the electromagnetic source of the CNV wave. Their experiment suggests that the terminal CNV is located within Brodmann's area 6 and corresponds to the premotor cortex.[12]

The work done by Zappoli and colleagues is another example of research completed to determine the generators of the CNV component. Zappoli (2003) studied the ERP patterns, including the CNV, of subjects with brain disorders or brain damage.[13] Zappoli reviews evidence which shows that in certain cases epileptic discharges affect the expectance waves and therefore decrease the CNV amplitude. Zappoli also described research which investigated the CNV characteristics in patients which had lobotomies of frontal regions. The CNV amplitudes were decreased or absent in these patients.

Theory[edit]

Many theories have been posited to account for cognitive processes underlying the CNV component. Walter and colleagues suggested that CNV amplitude varies directly with subjective probability or expectancy of the imperative stimuli. Other researchers suggested that the CNV amplitude varies with the intention to perform an act. Another theory is that CNV varies with the motivation of the subject to complete the task. Tecce suggests that the CNV is related to both attention and arousal level.

See also[edit]

  • C1 and P1
  • Difference due to memory
  • Early left anterior negativity
  • Error-related negativity
  • Late positive component
  • Lateralized readiness potential
  • Mismatch negativity
  • N2pc
  • N100
  • N170
  • N200
  • N400
  • P3a
  • P3b
  • P200
  • P300 (neuroscience)
  • P600
  • Somatosensory evoked potential
  • Visual N1
  • References[edit]

    1. ^ Hilmar Gudziol, Orlando Guntinas-Lichius. Chapter 16 - Electrophysiologic assessment of olfactory and gustatory function. In: Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Volume 164, 2019, Pages 247-262. Elsevier.
  • ^ a b c Walter, W.G; Cooper, R.; Aldridge, V.J.; McCallum, W.C.; Winter, A.L. (1964). "Contingent Negative Variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain". Nature. 203 (4943): 380–384. doi:10.1038/203380a0. PMID 14197376.
  • ^ L. Bozinovska, G. Stojanov, M. Sestakov, S. Bozinovski. CNV pattern recognition – a step toward cognitive wave observation. In: L. Torres, E. Masgrau, M. Lagunas, editors. Signal processing: theories and applications. Proceedings of the Fifth European Signal Processing Conference (EUSIPCO 90); 1990, Barcelona. 1990, Elsevier Science Publishers; 1990. p. 1659–1662
  • ^ L Bozinovska, S. Bozinovski, G. Stojanov. Electroexpectogram: experimental design and algorithms. Proceedings of the IEEE International. Biomedical. Engineering Days; 1992. Istanbul. p. 58–60
  • ^ A. Bozinovski, L. Bozinovska. Anticipatory Brain Potentials in a Brain-Robot Interface Paradigm. Proceedings of the 4th International IEEE EMBS Conference on Neural Engineering, Antalya, Turkey, p. 451-454, 2009
  • ^ a b c d e f g Tecce, J.J. (1972). "Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man". Psychological Bulletin. 77 (2): 73–108. doi:10.1037/h0032177. PMID 4621420.
  • ^ a b Frost, B.G.; Neill, R.A.; Fenelon, B. (1988). "The determinants of the non-motoric CNV in a complex, variable foreperiod, information processing paradigm". Biological Psychology. 27 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(88)90002-6. PMID 3251557.
  • ^ Loveless, N.E; Sanford, A.J. (1975). "The impact of warning signal intensity on reaction time and components of the contingent negative variation". Biological Psychology. 2 (3): 217–226. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(75)90021-6. PMID 1139019.
  • ^ Weerts, T.C.; Lang, P.J. (1973). "The effects of eye fixation and stimulus and response location on the contingent negative variation (CNV)". Biological Psychology. 1 (1): 1–19. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(73)90010-0. PMID 4804295.
  • ^ Gaillard, AW (1976). "Effects of warning-signal modality on the contingent negative variation (CNV)". Biological Psychology. 4 (2): 139–154. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(76)90013-2. PMID 1276304.
  • ^ Ruchkin, D.S.; Sutton, S.; Mahaffey, D.; Glaser, J. (1986). "Terminal CNV in the absence of motor response". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 63 (5): 445–463. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(86)90127-6. PMID 2420561.
  • ^ Hultin, L.; Rossini, P.; Romani, G. L.; Högstedt, P.; Tecchio, F.; Pizzella, V. (1996). "Neuromagnetic localization of the late component of the contingent negative variation". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 98: 425–448.
  • ^ Zappoli, R. (2003). "Permanent or transitory effects on neurocognitive components of the CNV complex induced by brain dysfunctions, lesions, and ablations in humans". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 48 (2): 189–220. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00054-0. PMID 12763574.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Contingent_negative_variation&oldid=1076838030"

    Categories: 
    Electroencephalography
    Evoked potentials
     



    This page was last edited on 13 March 2022, at 06:14 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki