Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Background  





2 First study  



2.1  Method  





2.2  Discussion  







3 Second study  



3.1  Method  





3.2  Discussion  







4 Reception  





5 Characteristics  





6 Explanations  



6.1  Disproved causes  



6.1.1  Mere exposure  





6.1.2  Subjective frequency  





6.1.3  Evaluative conditioning  





6.1.4  Subjective ownership  





6.1.5  Mastery pleasure  







6.2  Probable cause  



6.2.1  Mere ownership  





6.2.2  Implicit self-esteem  









7 Application  





8 Wider implications  



8.1  In the lab  





8.2  Outside the lab  



8.2.1  Controversial studies  





8.2.2  Undisputed studies  









9 Footnotes  





10 References  



10.1  Sources  
















Name-letter effect






العربية
Español
Bahasa Indonesia
Magyar
Nederlands
Polski
Українська

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 





Featured article

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


The name-letter effect is the tendency of people to prefer the letters in their name over other letters in the alphabet. Whether subjects are asked to rank all letters of the alphabet, rate each of the letters, choose the letter they prefer out of a set of two, or pick a small set of letters they most prefer, on average people consistently like the letters in their own name the most. Crucially, subjects are not aware that they are choosing letters from their name.

Discovered in 1985 by the Belgian psychologist Jozef Nuttin, the name-letter effect has been replicated in dozens of studies, involving subjects from over 15 countries, using four different alphabets. It holds across age and gender. People who changed their names many years ago tend to prefer the letters of both their current and original names over non-name letters. The effect is most prominent for initials, but even when initials are excluded, the remaining letters of both given and family names still tend to be preferred over non-name letters.

Most people like themselves; the name is associated with the self, and hence the letters of the name are preferred, despite the fact that they appear in many other words. People who do not like themselves tend not to exhibit the name-letter effect. A similar effect has been found for numbers related to birthdays: people tend to prefer the number signifying the day of the month on which they were born. Alternative explanations for the name-letter effect, such as frequent exposure and early mastery, have been ruled out. In psychological assessments, the Name Letter Preference Task is widely used to estimate implicit self-esteem.

There is some evidence that the effect has implications for real-life decisions. In the lab, people disproportionately favor brands matching their initials. An analysis of a large database of charity donations revealed that a disproportionately large number of people donate to disaster relief following hurricanes with names sharing their initial letter (e.g. Kate and Kevin following Hurricane Katrina). Studies that investigate the impact of name-letter matching on bigger life decisions (where to live, whom to marry, which occupation to take on) are controversial.

Background[edit]

Systematic interest in the letter preference began in 1959 with brand-preference studies by researchers Mecherikoff and Horton. These tried to find the relative appeal of letters for use in package labels.[1] In an extension of the studies, subjects were asked to rank the English alphabet by the pleasantness of the appearance of capital letters. While there was not a great deal of agreement amongst the subjects (the coefficients of concordance were low),[2] a strong positive correlation was found between a letter's average rank and how frequently it occurred as an initial letter of family names.[3]

Robert Zajonc, a social psychologist, published research in 1968 into preferences between pairs of words (e.g. "on" or "off"): in the overwhelming majority of trials the preferred word was also the most common.[4] Zajonc also tested preferences for nonsense words and found that people liked them the more they heard them.[5] He interpreted these results as evidence that mere repeated exposure to a stimulus is sufficient to enhance its attractiveness.[6]

Around 1977, Belgian experimental psychologist Jozef Nuttin was driving on a highway looking at license plates when he noticed that he preferred plates containing letters from his own name.[7] He wondered if people in general would prefer stimuli that are somehow connected to them; a "mere belongingness" as opposed to Zajonc's mere exposure.[7][8]

First study[edit]

In his lab at the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Nuttin designed experiments to test the hypothesis that people place a higher value on letters that feature in their name.[7][8] It was crucial to the experimental design to rule out other factors, particularly mere exposure. If letters in a name are also letters that occur with higher frequency, then a preference for one's own letters might arise from the mere-exposure effect.[8]

Method[edit]

First 11 stimuli for a yoke
Irma Maes Jef Jacobs
A U A U
M D M D
T R T R
I G I G
V S V S
E N E N
A P A P
L M L M
H F H F
E I E I
J K J K

To find an effect which ruled out mere exposure, Nuttin created a yoked control design in which two subjects evaluated the same letters separately. Some of the letters belonged to one subject's name, and some of the letters belonged to the other subject's name, while some were random. In this design, any difference in preference between subjects would have to be based on whether the letter occurred in their name.[9]

For example, take the fictitious pair Irma Maes and Jef Jacobs as shown in the table. The first stimulus is A and U: the last letter of Irma's first name and a letter not in her name. The next stimulus is M and D: the penultimate letter from Irma's first name and a letter not part of her name. As can be seen in the table this is repeated for the remaining letters of Irma's first name. The letters of her last name then also appear in reverse order, and finally the letters of both of Jef's names. The shading in the table reveals the pattern hidden to subjects, who would have been told to circle their preferred letter of each pair as fast as possible without thinking.[10]

In the first trial, 38 Dutch-speaking local elementary school girls circled the letters they preferred in two yoked lists of letter pairs. A significant preference for the letters of one's own name over those of the other person was found.[11] The second experiment used 98 Dutch-speaking local university students, to see if more years of reading made a difference. Four other factors were varied: either pairs or triads of letters; encircling the preferred letter or crossing out the less preferred ones; the letters QXYZ, infrequent in Dutch, included or excluded; own-name letters presented first or last.[11] All conditions gave a name-letter effect, with a stronger effect when QXYZ were included and the less preferred letter was crossed out.[12]Nosignificant difference was found using family name rather than first name or both names.[13] While the effect was strongest for initials, subsequent data analysis revealed a significant effect even without the first and last initials.[13]

Discussion[edit]

Nuttin concluded that the experiments showed that, independent of visual, acoustical, aesthetic, semantic, and frequency characteristics, letters belonging to one's own first and family names are preferred above other letters.[13] He framed the effect in the context of narcissism, Gestalt theory and awareness, as reflected in the title of his 1985 article "Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter effect", in which "beyond Gestalt" refers to the fact that subjects were not shown names, only letters in isolation, and "beyond awareness" to the fact that subjects did not realize that the letters of their own names were used.[14] Nuttin claimed the effect he found was the first to go beyond Gestalt and awareness.[15]

Second study[edit]

In 1987 Nuttin published his second study, describing experiments done in 1984 and 1985 with the help of Hilde Sas.[16][17] Because of the far-reaching implications of the name-letter effect for psychological theories, Nuttin found it wise to first test the effect's generality and robustness, before setting off on a research program aimed at understanding the underlying affective and cognitive processes at work. He wondered whether the effect would be found in all cultural and linguistic communities, or whether the first study revealed an effect due to some unknown idiosyncratic aspect of the Dutch language in Belgium.[18]

Method[edit]

Cross-lingual studies were performed at 13 European universities, using 12 different languages, viz. Dutch, English, Finnish, French, German, Greek (the only one with a non-Roman alphabet), Hungarian, Italian, Norwegian, Polish, Portuguese, and Spanish. Because the original yoked design did not lend itself well to long-distance research and standardization, it was replaced by a simpler, easier to replicate experimental design. Subjects were asked to mark the six capital letters they liked most in a randomized list containing all letters of the local alphabet, again without giving it much thought. They had to mark their first preference with 1, their second with 2, etc. The new method was first applied in Belgium. When results showed the name-letter effect at work again, it was copied in the other countries. A total of 2,047 subjects participated, all students.[19]

Discussion[edit]

Across languages and letters, the average probability of a letter being chosen as one of the six preferred letters was 0.30 for name letters and 0.20 for other letters. The strongest effects were observed in the Norwegian and Finnish studies. In the Hungarian, Portuguese, and Italian studies the effect was present but not to a significant degree.[20] The effect was also found when only looking at letters in family names, as well as only first name letters. The name-letter effect emerged as very significant in all languages when only initials were considered.[21] There was a probability of 0.46 that initial letters were chosen amongst the top-six letters. Further analysis revealed that the overall name-letter effect is not simply due to initials: when excluding initials a name-letter effect was still found across all languages.[22]

Nuttin analyzed the data to see if there was a national-letter effect, but failed to find one. Norwegians did not favor the letter N (for Norge) more than people from other countries did, neither did the Hungarians favor the letter M (for Magyarország). This led Nuttin to conclude that individual ownership has affective consequences that are not observed for collective ownership.[23]

The data also allowed for an investigation into whether visual prominence is an important factor in the name-letter effect. Cars in Austria and Hungary have a sticker displaying their nationality with a capital letter that does not match the country's name in the local language (A and H respectively). This did not have any impact on people in those countries liking those letters relatively more.[24]

As in the first study, the second one also included a task relating to disliking letters. Subjects were asked to select the six letters they liked the least. As before, merely having a letter in one's own name significantly reduced the chances of disliking it.[25] This task revealed an asymmetry in the letter preference hierarchy. While there was a large consensus within each of the 12 languages as to which letters were least preferred, there was not much consensus at all around the most preferred letters.[26][A]

Reception[edit]

In the light of how surprising the finding was, Nuttin hesitated for seven years before finally going public with it. He first mentioned it at a conference of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology in 1984,[28] followed by the 1985 and 1987 articles referred to above. His work was met with widespread skepticism, as he had expected.[29] Loosen, a researcher at Nuttin's own university, called the name-letter effect "so strange that a down-to-earth researcher will spontaneously think of an artifact".[30][B] Other researchers did not explicitly say that the effect was spurious, but they doubted its psychological relevance.[29] In the first five years after publication (1985–1989), Nuttin's 1985 article was cited only once and the effect was studied at only one other university (Ohio State University, where Johnson replicated the effect using American students).[31][32][C]

This all changed in 1995, when Greenwald and Banaji pointed out that Nuttin's work was relevant to indirect measurement of self-esteem, which Nuttin himself had actually already suggested.[29][34] After that the original study was cited 14 times in the five years between 1995 and 1999, 50 times between 2000 and 2004, 114 times between 2005 and 2009, and approximately 200 times between 2010 and 2014.[29] The name-letter effect is no longer disputed and Nuttin's work has been called "seminal" by Stieger, Voracek, and Formann in their 2012 meta-analysis of 44 publications on the effect.[35] Their meta-analysis found no trace of publication bias.[36]

Characteristics[edit]

In her 2014 meta-analysis of dozens of name-letter effect studies, Hoorens called the effect robust.[31] She noted robustness in:

Explanations[edit]

Various explanations for the name-letter effect have been explored. Several explanations which seemed plausible at first have since been rejected.

Disproved causes[edit]

Mere exposure[edit]

People may simply like most what they see most. Letters that appear more frequently in everyday usage also occur more often in people's names. Forer, in 1940, and Alluisi and Adams, in 1962, found a positive correlation between the frequency of occurrence of letters and phonemes and how attractive they were judged to be.[47][48][49] Zajonc extended these studies, using foreign symbols and controlling the number of exposures. This led him to formulate the mere-exposure hypothesis: the more something is seen, the more it is liked.[50] Nuttin's original study showed that mere exposure can not be the cause of the name-letter effect, as letters with equal frequency were evaluated differently by people who had the letter in their names and those who did not.[15][H] He also found that whereas the letter B is of low frequency in French and the letter Q of medium frequency, in experiments with French speakers the B was among the most highly liked letters and the Q was among the least liked. Similarly, in Polish the Y is a medium frequency letter, yet was still among the least preferred letters.[51] In each of the languages, the least frequent letters were over-represented in the set of least preferred letters, whereas the most preferred letters were rarely the highest frequency letters. The exposure counts in the lab studies done by Zajonc (20 to 25 times) were minuscule compared to real-life observation counts of any letter, which also caused Nuttin to cast doubt on whether Zajonc's theory could hold true in the real world.[52]

Subjective frequency[edit]

Subjective frequency is how frequently subjects think a stimulus appears. Name letters may be noticed more and, consequently, assumed to occur more frequently than other letters. Early research into the impact of exposure showed that differences in subjective frequency yielded different results even when the actual frequency was identical.[53] Hoorens and Nuttin tested whether subjective frequency could be an explanation for the name-letter effect by asking subjects to rank-order the entire alphabet twice, once according to their letter preference and once according to estimated letter frequency. Subjects indeed significantly overestimated the frequency of letters in their names, although there was no significant positive correlation between the overestimation of frequency and the name-letter effect.[54] The researchers also asked subjects to rate how much they liked their own name. Subjects who liked their name had a stronger name-letter effect than those who did not like their name, but they did not overestimate the frequency of own-name letters more than subjects who did not like their names. Hoorens and Nuttin concluded that there is no support for the subjective frequency hypothesis.[54]

Evaluative conditioning[edit]

Evaluative conditioning suggests that if the name is liked then the name letters will be liked too. This would occur through repeated visual association of the name letters with the name. Martin and Levey defined evaluative conditioning as a variation of classical conditioning in which we come to like or dislike something through an association.[55] Given the observation that our own name stands out among others as quite an attractive stimulus, as Cherry found in the cocktail party effect, it could be that the name-letter effect results from evaluative conditioning.[56][57] Feys set up a controlled study with Flemish subjects, pairing unfamiliar symbols (Japanese kanji) with subjects' own names, and with other names. He found that there was no difference in how much subjects liked the kanji symbol representing their own name or other names. He concluded that evaluative conditioning is not the primary cause of the name-letter effect.[56]

Subjective ownership[edit]

Subjective ownership would occur if subjects knowingly chose the letters from their own name. Nuttin ruled out a conscious response strategy in tests. Despite being given a monetary reward and unlimited time, none of the subjects of his original study were able to find a pattern in the stimulus lists, ruling out the possibility that they knew their own name-letters were there.[58]

Mastery pleasure[edit]

The letters first learned by a child, commonly their own name,[59] may come to have lasting positive associations.[60] Hoorens and Todorova tested this by looking for a name-letter effect in bilingual subjects where their mother tongue alphabet was Cyrillic and their foreign-language alphabet Roman.[61] Because learning a foreign language at a later age does not typically involve extra attention given to name letters, there should be no name-letter effect in the foreign-language alphabet, only in the first-language alphabet.[60] Results of a study with 100 Bulgarian subjects who at a later age learned English, German, French or Spanish revealed a name-letter effect for the Roman alphabet as well as for Cyrillic.[62][I] The researchers concluded mastery pleasure is not the principal cause of the name-letter effect.[63] In a follow-up study Hoorens, Nuttin, Herman and Pavakanun tested the strength of the name-letter effect among elementary-school children, in a cross-sectional experimental design involving Flemish and Hungarian second, fourth, and sixth graders. Instead of finding the name-effect to decrease with age as might be expected, they found it increased, thus proving that mastery pleasure is not the principal cause.[64][J] They also investigated name-letter effects in bilingual Thai subjects, some of whom had learned the English alphabet at the same time as the Thai alphabet, and others who had learned it later.[K] They found that the time at which students had learned the second alphabet made no difference in the strength of the name-letter effect, thereby ruling out mastery pleasure as a co-determinant.[67]

Probable cause[edit]

The effect is thought to arise from unconscious, automatic processes of self-evaluation, with different research groups coming at it from two different angles.[15][68]

Mere ownership[edit]

Nuttin frames the cause in terms of ownership, which has roots in economic psychology. The endowment effect has found that people ascribe more value to things merely because they own them.[69][70] Nuttin puts the name-letter effect down to people automatically liking and valuing anything that is connected to them. As such, the name-letter effect is just one example of a more general mere-ownership effect.[15] In which case, various verifiable predictions follow.

Implicit self-esteem[edit]

Another group of researchers has framed the cause in terms of social psychology, specifically the self-esteem theory. Beginning with Greenwald and Banaji in 1995, this group states that the name-letter effect results from implicit self-esteem, a person's tendency to evaluate him or herself positively in a spontaneous, automatic, or unconscious manner.[74][68] Most people like themselves.[75] The halo effect of self-esteem spreads to any attributes associated with the self, including the name and its letters.[76] As early as 1926 Syz discovered that a person's own name is special compared to others, eliciting physical responses measurable on the skin.[77] It is thought that when a person recognizes the letters in his or her name, that person experiences positive feelings of implicit self-esteem. These positive feelings induce subjects to unknowingly select the letters of their own name, producing the name-letter effect.[78]

Application[edit]

Inpsychological assessments, the name-letter effect has been exploited to measure self-esteem.[90] There are two types of self-esteem: explicit self-esteem (a person's deliberate and conscious evaluation of themselves) and implicit self-esteem.[74] Because by definition implicit self-esteem is not accessible to introspection, measures of it do not rely on direct self-reports but on the degree to which objects associated with the self generate positive versus negative thoughts.[91][92]

The Letter Preference Task is the second-most popular method to measure implicit self-esteem, surpassed only by the Implicit Association Test.[7] The task has also been called the Name Letter Preference Task, Name-Letter-Ratings Measure, and Initial Preference Task.[35][93] There is no standard method for applying the task. The most commonly used one is a letter rating task, which involves having participants judge all the letters of the alphabet. Even within this method there are variations in the instructions (how much you like the letters or how attractive you find them), in the rating scales (five-point, seven-point, or nine-point), in the order of the letters (random or alphabetical), and in data collection (paper-and-pencil or computer-based).[7]

There is no standard algorithm for calculating implicit self-esteem. At least six algorithms are in use.[94] In their meta-analysis of the name-letter effect, Stieger, Voracek, and Formann recommend using the ipsatized double-correction algorithm (the so-called "I-algorithm"), as originally recommended and named by LeBel and Gawronski[95]).[94] In her meta-analysis, Hoorens does not recommend a specific algorithm as little is known about how name-letter preference scores obtained from different algorithms relate to the most important psychometric quality of all, validity.[96] The algorithms are typically applied to initials only, but can be used for all name letters.[29]

Stieger, Voracek, and Formann recommend that the task be administered twice, that the effects be calculated separately for first-name initial and last-name initial, that the task be accompanied with the birthday-number task, and that the instructions focus on liking rather than attractiveness.[97] They suggest it may be useful to use not just initials but all name letters for measuring implicit self-esteem, something which Hoorens says is her most important recommendation.[97][98] The Letter Preference Task has been used to measure implicit self-esteem in contexts as diverse as depression, physical health, social acceptance, unrealistic optimism, feedback sensitivity, self-regulation, and defensiveness.[99]

Wider implications[edit]

Researchers have looked for wider implications of the name-letter effect on preferences, both inside and outside the lab.

In the lab[edit]

In controlled studies in the lab, Hodson and Olson tried to find evidence of people liking everyday things (e.g. foods, animals) that matched their name initials. No evidence was found, neither for a between-subjects effect (e.g. Judy did not like things starting with J, such as jam, more than Doug liked things starting with J), nor for a within-individuals effect (e.g. Judy did not like jam more than honey).[100] The researchers did discover a small but reliable effect of initials on brand-name preferences within individuals (e.g. Hank did like Honda more than non-matching brands).[101] They speculated that brand names are more likely to communicate identity to others than other everyday things.[102] Stieger extended this research by looking at buying preferences for product names. He found that people were disproportionately more likely to buy products matching their initials.[103] The effect mainly occurred for big brands. No correlation was found between the strength of an individual's name-letter effect and the strength of his or her name-letter-branding effect.[104]

Wiebenga and Fennis investigated whether the use of the personal pronouns "I" and "my" in branding also had an effect on preferences, given the way these pronouns link to the self. They found that brand names with a generic self-referencing pronoun (e.g. iPhone, Myspace) were evaluated more positively than non-self-referencing brand names (e.g. Xbox). The effect behaved like the name-letter effect: when the self was put under threat the effect became stronger, and it disappeared for people with negative self-evaluations.[105]

A study by Polman, Pollmann, and Poehlman found that sharing initials with members in a group can increase the quality of group work. In a study of undergraduate students they found that groups sharing initials performed better than groups that did not. Groups that had a higher proportion of shared initials exceeded groups with a lower proportion of shared initials.[106]

Outside the lab[edit]

Controversial studies[edit]

Researchers have looked for evidence of the driving force behind the name-letter effect outside the lab. A body of controversial research under the umbrella of implicit egotism, starting with Pelham, Mirenberg, and Jones, has investigated whether people unknowingly make life decisions based on their name letters or name.[107][108][109]  Sceptics say that a claim that name letters influence life decisions is an extraordinary claim that requires extraordinary evidence.[110]

Simonsohn urged researchers to look hard for confounding variables in field data analysis. An example of a confounding variable is baby-name popularity. This has fluctuated significantly over the decades. Simonsohn found that although Walter and Dennis are of near-identical popularity in a large sample of US first names of living people, Walter is a relatively old-fashioned name. He suggested that when Pelham et al. found a disproportionately high number of dentists whose name started with the letters "Den" compared to with "Wal", they had overlooked that people named Walter would tend to be old, and more likely to be retired. Baby-name fluctuation seemed a better explanation for disproportionately more "Den" dentists than "Wal" than implicit egotism.[111] Using both a different data set (Twitter and Google+ accounts) and a different statistical analysis, Kooti, Magno, and Weber found no evidence of people disproportionately having a job matching their name initials.[112][L]

Dyjas, Grasman, Wetzels, van der Maas, and Wagenmakers criticized the method Pelham et al. used in their analysis of archives of deaths in 23 "Saint cities" in the US, such as St. Louis and St. Paul. Whereas Pelham et al. pooled all data together and concluded that people gravitate towards cities matching their first name (e.g. Louis or Paul), Dyjas et al. used Bayesian hierarchical modelling to show that there are several cities where the opposite happens, people moving away from their name Saint city. They concluded that there is no evidence for an overall effect.[117] A different set of cities containing 30 surnames, such as Jackson and Johnson City, did reveal a disproportionately large number of deceased people with city–surname matches.[118] Dyjas et al. disputed that people gravitate towards cities of their surname, but instead cited Simonsohn's argument that many descendants of founders of these cities may never have moved away, a case of reverse causality.[119][120]

Simonsohn also raised the possibility of reverse causality in the case of Anseel and Duyck's analysis of a large data set consisting of Belgians' last names and the companies they work for. Anseel and Duyck concluded that people tend to choose to work for companies that match their initial.[121] But Simonsohn suspected that, like Walt Disney working for Disney, many people work for companies named after themselves or a family member. When he controlled for reverse causality in a large US data set, he could not see any evidence for people choosing to work for companies matching their initial.[122]

A few controversial studies have linked performance to initials. However, McCullough and Williams found no evidence of a name-letter effect for the letter 'K' in baseball players striking out (shown on the scoreboard with a 'K'), despite an earlier study by Nelson and Simmons suggesting there was.[123][124] Nelson and Simmons also found that students with first name initials C or D get lower grades than others with initials A or B.[124] Again, McCullough and Williams criticized the statistical analysis used and found no evidence to support such a relation.[125]

In response to Simonsohn's critical analyses of their earlier methods, Pelham and Carvallo published a new study in 2015, describing how they now controlled for gender, ethnicity, and education confounds. In one study they looked at census data and concluded that men disproportionately worked in eleven occupations that matched their surnames, for example, Baker, Carpenter, and Farmer, something the New Scientist has coined nominative determinism.[108][126] Voracek, Rieder, Stieger, and Swami investigated which way the arrow of causality points when it comes to names influencing choice of occupation. They reported that today's Smiths still tend to have the physical capabilities of their ancestors who were smiths. In the researchers' view a genetic-social hypothesis appeared more viable than the hypothesis of implicit egotism effects.[127]

Undisputed studies[edit]

Satellite image of a hurricane
Hurricane Katrina: people with the initial K especially donated to the disaster relief.

Research by Chandler, Griffin, and Sorensen into a link between letter names and donations has been widely accepted. They analyzed the records of disaster relief donations after seven hurricanes (among others, Katrina, Rita and Mitch). They found that people who shared an initial with the hurricane were overrepresented as donors. They concluded that people want to overcome some of the negative feelings associated with the shared name and thus donate.[128][129] Simonsohn suggested that implicit egotism only applies to cases where people are nearly indifferent between options, and therefore it would not apply to major decisions such as career choices, but would to low-stakes decisions such as choosing a charity.[130]

Footnotes[edit]

  1. ^ This asymmetry is similar to what Czapiński found when analyzing school-trip companion preferences: much consensus as to who were the three least preferred students, but little in who were the three most preferred.[27]
  • ^ Loosen's critique stemmed from misinterpreting Nuttin's experimental design. Loosen thought that subjects in the yoked experiments were randomly selected to form a pair. However, this had not been the case. Nuttin had selected pairs based on minimal overlap of syllables in the subjects' names.[29]
  • ^ Because the effect is most profound for initials, Johnson had christened the effect the initial-letter effect. Nuttin did not agree with this reductionism, saying there is no need to create the illusion of two phenomena where there is only one.[33]
  • ^ Koole, Smeets, van Knippenberg, and Dijksterhuis found an effect only for initials, not for letters in other positions.[37]
  • ^ The exceptions are a study by Albers, Rotteveel, and Dijksterhuis, and one by Stieger, Preyss, and Voracek.[38][39]
  • ^ Gender-role orientation, the extent to which an individual adopts and displays traits, attitudes, and behaviors normatively identified as male-typical or female-typical, may also play a part in the name-letter effect.[40] It can be measured by the gender initial-preference task, which requires participants to rate letters for their gender typicality. Men have been shown to rate their initial letters as more male-typical, whereas women rate their initials as more female-typical.[41]
  • ^ The only known exception is a study by Kernis, Lakey, and Heppner.[42]
  • ^ Nuttin made the assumption that for any given letter, total exposure has been roughly the same for each subject. That is, an adult's name is read or written infrequently compared to all other words.[15] Other researchers agreed; Greenwald and Banaji spoke of "reaching an asymptote".[44]
  • ^ The effect was not due to the two alphabets sharing letters. It was found for common letters with identical pronunciation (AEKMT), common letters with different pronunciation (BCFPYU), and Roman capitals not appearing in Cyrillic (DFGIJNRVWZ).[62]
  • ^ Other studies have since also found the effect to increase over age, for instance, Corenblum and Armstrong in their study of Native Canadian children.[65]
  • ^ Only Thai consonants were presented as stimuli. Thai vowels can not be used in isolation as their sound depends on their context.[66]
  • ^ Overlooking a cohort confound was also the problem with controversial research that tied initials to death. Christenfeld, Phillips, and Glynn concluded that people who have positive monograms (e.g. ACE or VIP) live significantly longer than those with negative initials (e.g. PIG or DIE). This conclusion was based on analysis of thousands of California death certificates between 1969 and 1995.[113] Morrison and Smith subsequently pointed out that this was an artefact of grouping data by age at death. Frequency of initials changing over time could be a confounding variable. When they grouped the same data by birth year, they found no statistically significant relationship between initials and longevity.[114] Similarly, Smith found statistical errors had led Abel and Kruger wrongly to conclude that Major League Baseball players whose names have positive initials live longer than players with negative initials.[115][116]
  • References[edit]

  • ^ Horton & Mecherikoff 1960, p. 253.
  • ^ Alluisi & Adams 1962, pp. 124–125.
  • ^ Zajonc 1968, pp. 6–7.
  • ^ Zajonc 1968, p. 24.
  • ^ Zajonc 1968, p. 1.
  • ^ a b c d e Hoorens 2014, p. 230.
  • ^ a b c Nuttin 1985, p. 354.
  • ^ Nuttin 1985, pp. 354–355.
  • ^ Nuttin 1985, pp. 354–356.
  • ^ a b Nuttin 1985, p. 356.
  • ^ Nuttin 1985, p. 357.
  • ^ a b c Nuttin 1985, p. 358.
  • ^ Nuttin 1985, pp. 358–359.
  • ^ a b c d e Nuttin 1985, p. 359.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 381.
  • ^ Sas 1986.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 384.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, pp. 385–386.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 390.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 391.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 392.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, pp. 396–397.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 397.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, pp. 397–398.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 398.
  • ^ Czapiński 1980, p. 203.
  • ^ Nuttin 1984.
  • ^ a b c d e f Hoorens 2014, p. 234.
  • ^ Loosen 1990, p. 11.
  • ^ a b c d e f g Hoorens 2014, p. 235.
  • ^ Johnson 1986.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 396.
  • ^ Greenwald & Banaji 1995, p. 11.
  • ^ a b Stieger, Voracek & Formann 2012, p. 63.
  • ^ Stieger, Voracek & Formann 2012, pp. 66–67.
  • ^ Koole et al. 1999, p. 111.
  • ^ Albers, Rotteveel & Dijksterhuis 2009, p. 63.
  • ^ Stieger, Preyss & Voracek 2012, p. 51.
  • ^ Stieger et al. 2014, p. 358.
  • ^ Stieger et al. 2014.
  • ^ Kernis, Lakey & Heppner 2008, p. 477.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, p. 204.
  • ^ a b c d Kitayama & Karasawa 1997, p. 740.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, p. 184.
  • ^ Stieger & LeBel 2012, p. 10.
  • ^ Hoorens & Nuttin 1993, p. 181.
  • ^ Forer 1940, p. 589.
  • ^ Alluisi & Adams 1962, p. 123.
  • ^ Zajonc 1980, p. 151.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, p. 399.
  • ^ Nuttin 1987, pp. 398–400.
  • ^ Matlin 1971, p. 295.
  • ^ a b Hoorens & Nuttin 1993, p. 177.
  • ^ Martin & Levey 1987, p. 69.
  • ^ a b Feys 1995, p. 560.
  • ^ Mack & Rock 1998, p. 18.
  • ^ Hoorens 1990, p. 29.
  • ^ Treiman & Broderick 1998, p. 112.
  • ^ a b Hoorens & Todorova 1988, p. 366.
  • ^ Hoorens & Todorova 1988, p. 365.
  • ^ a b c Hoorens & Todorova 1988, p. 367.
  • ^ Hoorens & Todorova 1988, p. 368.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, p. 190.
  • ^ Corenblum & Armstrong 2012, p. 130.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, p. 192.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, pp. 196–201.
  • ^ a b Koole 2007, p. 603.
  • ^ Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193.
  • ^ Beggan 1992, p. 229.
  • ^ Hoorens & Nuttin 1993, pp. 177–200.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 1990, pp. 183–184.
  • ^ Nickell, Pederson & Rossow 2003, pp. 161–163.
  • ^ a b Greenwald & Banaji 1995, pp. 4, 10–11.
  • ^ Greenwald & Banaji 1995, p. 10.
  • ^ Greenwald & Banaji 1995, pp. 10–11.
  • ^ Syz 1926, pp. 119–126.
  • ^ Koole & Pelham 2003, p. 108.
  • ^ Koole, Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg 2001, pp. 672–673.
  • ^ Hoorens et al. 2015, p. 910.
  • ^ Koole, Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg 2001, p. 673.
  • ^ Wilson et al. 1989, p. 330.
  • ^ Koole, Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg 2001, pp. 674–676.
  • ^ Jones et al. 2002, p. 170.
  • ^ Komori & Murata 2008, p. 109.
  • ^ Wentura, Kulfanek & Greve 2001, p. 657.
  • ^ Koole & Pelham 2003, pp. 101–102.
  • ^ LeBel & Campbell 2009, p. 1293.
  • ^ DeHart et al. 2011, p. 127.
  • ^ Stieger, Voracek & Formann 2012, p. 64.
  • ^ Spalding & Hardin 1999, p. 535.
  • ^ Krizan & Suls 2008, p. 522.
  • ^ Hoorens 2014, p. 233.
  • ^ a b Stieger, Voracek & Formann 2012, p. 71.
  • ^ LeBel & Gawronski 2009, p. 101.
  • ^ Hoorens 2014, p. 244.
  • ^ a b Stieger, Voracek & Formann 2012, p. 76.
  • ^ Hoorens 2014, p. 248.
  • ^ LeBel & Gawronski 2009, p. 86.
  • ^ Hodson & Olson 2005, p. 1099.
  • ^ Hodson & Olson 2005, p. 1108.
  • ^ Hodson & Olson 2005, p. 1109.
  • ^ Stieger 2010, p. 1089.
  • ^ Stieger 2010, p. 1095.
  • ^ Wiebenga & Fennis 2012, p. 733.
  • ^ Polman, Pollmann & Poehlman 2013.
  • ^ Pelham, Mirenberg & Jones 2002, p. 479.
  • ^ a b Pelham & Carvallo 2015, p. 692.
  • ^ Jones et al. 2004, p. 665.
  • ^ Danesi 2012, p. 84.
  • ^ Simonsohn 2011a, p. 22–23.
  • ^ Kooti, Magno & Weber 2014, p. 216.
  • ^ Christenfeld, Phillips & Glynn 1999, p. 241.
  • ^ Morrison & Smith 2005, p. 820.
  • ^ Smith 2011, p. 211.
  • ^ Abel & Kruger 2007, p. 179.
  • ^ Dyjas et al. 2012, pp. 7–10.
  • ^ Dyjas et al. 2012, pp. 8–9.
  • ^ Dyjas et al. 2012, p. 9.
  • ^ Simonsohn 2011a, p. 15.
  • ^ Anseel & Duyck 2008, p. 1059.
  • ^ Simonsohn 2011b, p. 1087.
  • ^ McCullough & Williams 2010, p. 881.
  • ^ a b Nelson & Simmons 2007, p. 1106.
  • ^ McCullough & Williams 2011, p. 340.
  • ^ Feedback 1994.
  • ^ Voracek et al. 2015.
  • ^ Chandler, Griffin & Sorensen 2008, pp. 404–410.
  • ^ Simonsohn 2011a, pp. 44–45.
  • ^ Simonsohn 2011a, p. 46.
  • Sources[edit]

    • Abel, Ernest L.; Kruger, Michael L. (2007). "Symbolic significance of initials on longevity". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 104 (1): 179–182. doi:10.2466/pms.104.1.179-182. PMID 17450979. S2CID 32400715.(subscription required)
  • Albers, L.; Rotteveel, M.; Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). "Towards optimizing the name letter test as a measure of implicit self-esteem". Self and Identity. 8: 63–77. doi:10.1080/15298860802091062. hdl:2066/77250. S2CID 145104484.(subscription required)
  • Alluisi, E. A.; Adams, O. S. (1962). "Predicting letter preferences: Aesthetics and filtering in man". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 14: 123–131. doi:10.2466/pms.1962.14.1.123. S2CID 146381583.(subscription required)
  • Anseel, Frederik; Duyck, W. (2008). "Unconscious applicants: A systematic test of the name-letter effect". Psychological Science. 19 (10): 1059–1061. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02199.x. PMID 19000218. S2CID 11868804.Limited access icon
  • Beggan, James K. (1992). "On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 62 (2): 229–237. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.229.(subscription required)
  • Chandler, Jesse; Griffin, Tiffany M.; Sorensen, Nicholas (2008). "In the "I" of the storm: Shared initials increase disaster donations" (PDF). Judgment and Decision Making. 3 (5): 404–410. doi:10.1017/S1930297500000425. S2CID 14633255.Open access icon
  • Christenfeld, N.; Phillips, D. P.; Glynn, L. M. (1999). "What's in a name: Mortality and the power of symbols". Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 47 (3): 241–254. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00035-5. PMID 10576473.Limited access icon
  • Corenblum, B.; Armstrong, Helen D. (2012). "Racial-ethnic identity development in children in a racial-ethnic minority group". Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 44 (2): 124–137. doi:10.1037/a0027154.(subscription required)
  • Czapiński, Janusz (1980). "Positive–negative asymmetry on the group level". Polish Psychological Bulletin. 11 (3): 203–205 – via ResearchGate.
  • Danesi, Marcel (2012). Linguistic anthropology: A brief introduction. Toronto: Canadian Scholars' Press. ISBN 978-1-55130-489-2.
  • DeHart, Tracy; Pelham, Brett; Fiedorowicz, Luke; Carvallo, Mauricio; Gabriel, Shira (2011). "Including others in the implicit self: Implicit evaluation of significant others". Self and Identity. 10 (1): 127–135. doi:10.1080/15298861003687880. S2CID 144602581 – via Google Books.Limited access icon
  • Dyjas, Oliver; Grasman, Raoul P. P. P.; Wetzels, Ruud; van der Maas, Han L. J.; Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan (2012). "What's in a name: a Bayesian hierarchical analysis of the name–letter effect". Frontiers in Psychology. 3: 334. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00334. PMC 3457077. PMID 23055989.
  • Feedback (December 17, 1994). "Feedback". New Scientist (1956). Archived from the original on September 18, 2016.
  • Feys, J. (1995). "Mere ownership: Affective self-bias or evaluative conditioning?". European Journal of Social Psychology. 25 (5): 559–575. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420250507.(subscription required)
  • Forer, B. R. (1940). "A study of consonant preferences". Psychological Bulletin. 37 (8): 589. doi:10.1037/h0053393.
  • Greenwald, A. G.; Banaji, M R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.411.2919. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162. S2CID 8194189.
  • Hodson, G.; Olson, J. M. (2005). "Testing the generality of the name letter effect: Name initials and everyday attitudes". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 6 (8): 33–37. doi:10.1177/0146167205274895. PMID 6000270. S2CID 23154149.(subscription required)
  • Hoorens, Vera; Todorova, Elka (1988). "The name letter effect: Attachment to self or primacy of own name writing?". European Review of Social Psychology. 18 (4): 365–368. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420180406 – via Limo.
  • Hoorens, Vera (1990). "Nuttin's affective selfparticles hypothesis and the name letter effect: A review". Psychologica Belgica. 30 (1–2): 23–48. doi:10.5334/pb.801.
  • Hoorens, Vera; Nuttin, Jozef M. Jr.; Herman, I.E.; Pavakanun, U. (1990). "Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership: A quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross-alphabetical test of the name letter effect". European Review of Social Psychology. 20 (3): 181–205. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420200302.
  • Hoorens, Vera; Nuttin, Jozef M. Jr. (1993). "Overvaluation of own attributes: Mere ownership or subjective frequency?" (PDF). Social Cognition. 11 (2): 177–200. doi:10.1521/soco.1993.11.2.177.
  • Hoorens, Vera (2014). "What's really in a name-letter effect? Name-letter preferences as indirect measures of self-esteem". European Review of Social Psychology. 25 (1): 228–262. doi:10.1080/10463283.2014.980085. S2CID 144292531.
  • Hoorens, V.; Takano, K.; Franck, E.; Roberts, J.E.; Raes, F. (2015). "Initial and noninitial name-letter preferences as obtained through repeated letter rating tasks continue to reflect (different aspects of) self-esteem". Psychological Assessment. 27 (3): 905–914. doi:10.1037/pas0000092. hdl:10067/1268890151162165141. PMID 25730161.
  • Horton, David L.; Mecherikoff, Michael (1960). "Letter preferences: Ranking the alphabet". Journal of Applied Psychology. 44 (4): 252–253. doi:10.1037/h0045399.
  • Johnson, M. S. (1986). The initial letter effect: ego-attachment or mere exposure?. PhD dissertation. Columbus: The Ohio State University.
  • Jones, John T.; Pelham, Brett W.; Mirenberg, Matthew C.; Hetts, John J. (2002). "Name letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism as self-regulation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 38 (2): 170–177. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1497.
  • Jones, John T.; Pelham, Brett W.; Carvallo, Mauricio; Mirenberg, Matthew C. (2004). "How do I love thee? Let me count the Js: Implicit egotism and interperson attraction". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (5): 665–683. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.468.9494. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.665. PMID 15535778.
  • Kahneman, D.; Knetsch, J. L.; Thaler, R. H. (1991). "Anomalies: The endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias". Journal of Economic Perspectives. 5 (1): 193–206. doi:10.1257/jep.5.1.193.
  • Kernis, M. H.; Lakey, C. E.; Heppner, W. L. (2008). "Secure versus fragile high self-esteem as a predictor of verbal defensiveness: Converging findings across three different markers". Journal of Personality. 76 (3): 477–512. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.512.462. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00493.x. PMID 18447858.
  • Kitayama, Shinobu; Karasawa, Mayumi (1997). "Implicit self-esteem in Japan: Name letters and birthday numbers". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 23 (7): 736–742. doi:10.1177/0146167297237006. S2CID 143602622.
  • Komori, Megumi; Murata, Koji (2008). "Implicit egotism in Japan: Preference for first and family name initials". Hitotsubashi Journal of Social Studies. 40 (2): 101–109.
  • Koole, Sander L.; Smeets, Karianne; van Knippenberg, Ad; Dijksterhuis, Ap (1999). "The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (1): 111–125. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.111.
  • Koole, Sander L.; Dijksterhuis, Ap; van Knippenberg, Ad (2001). "What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (4): 669–685. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.669. hdl:1871/17530. PMID 11316229.
  • Koole, Sander L.; Pelham, Brett W. (2003). "On the nature of implicit self-esteem: The case of the name letter effect". In Steven Spencer; Steven Fein; Mark P. Zanna; James M. Olson (eds.). Motivated Social Perception. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 93–116. ISBN 978-0-8058-4036-0.
  • Koole, Sander (2007). "Name letter effect". In Baumeister, Roy; Vohs, Kathleen (eds.). Encyclopedia of Social Psychology. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. ISBN 978-1-4129-1670-7.
  • Kooti, Farshad; Magno, Gabriel; Weber, Ingmar (2014). "The social name-letter effect on online social networks". In Aiello, Luca Maria; McFarland, Daniel (eds.). Social Informatics - Proceedings of SocInfo 2014, Barcelona, Spain, November 11-13, 2014. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. pp. 216–227. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.763.4. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-13734-6_15. ISBN 978-3-319-13733-9. S2CID 10177951.
  • Krizan, Zlatan; Suls, Jerry (2008). "Are implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem related? A meta-analysis for the Name-Letter Test". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 521–531. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.017.
  • LeBel, E. P.; Campbell, L. (2009). "Implicit partner affect, relationship satisfaction, and the prediction of romantic breakup". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 45 (6): 1291–1294. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.003.
  • LeBel, Etienne P.; Gawronski, Bertram (2009). "How to find what's in a name: Scrutinizing the optimality of five scoring algorithms for the name-letter task". European Journal of Personality. 23 (2): 85–106. doi:10.1002/per.705. S2CID 53500983.
  • Loosen, F. (1990). The name letter effect: A fascinating idolum. Leuven: Peeters.
  • Mack, Arien; Rock, Irvin (1998). Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-13339-5.
  • Martin, I.; Levey, A. B. (1987). "Learning what will happen next: Conditioning, evaluation, and cognitive processes". In Davey, G. (ed.). Cognitive processes and Pavlovian conditioning in humans. Chichester, England: Wiley. pp. 57–81. ISBN 978-0-471-90791-6.
  • Matlin, Margaret W. (1971). "Response competition, recognition, and affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 19 (3): 295–300. doi:10.1037/h0031352. PMID 5120020.
  • McCullough, B. D.; Williams, Thomas P. (2010). "Baseball players with the initial "K" do not strike out more often". Journal of Applied Statistics. 37 (6): 881–891. Bibcode:2010JApSt..37..881M. doi:10.1080/02664760902889965. S2CID 20236883.
  • McCullough, B. D.; Williams, Thomas P. (2011). "Students with the initial "A" don't get better grades". Journal of Research in Personality. 45 (3): 340–343. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.03.006. ISSN 0092-6566.
  • Mecherikoff, Michael; Horton, David L. (1959). "Preferences for letters of the alphabet". Journal of Applied Psychology. 43 (2): 114–116. doi:10.1037/h0049029.
  • Morrison, Stilian; Smith, Gary (2005). "Monogrammatic determinism?". Psychosomatic Medicine. 67 (5): 820–824. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000181283.51771.8a. PMID 16204444. S2CID 31044960.
  • Nelson, Lief; Simmons, Joseph P. (2007). "Moniker maladies: When names sabotage success". Psychological Science. 18 (12): 1106–1112. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02032.x. PMID 18031419. S2CID 14274286.
  • Nickell, Gary; Pederson, Katie; Rossow, Cassie (2003). "The birthdate effect: An extension of the mere ownership effect". Psychological Reports. 92 (1): 161–163. doi:10.2466/PR0.92.1.161-163. PMID 12674276.
  • Nuttin, Jozef M. Jr. (1984). "What's in a name?". Opening Lecture of the 7th General Meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology.
  • Nuttin, Jozef M. Jr. (1985). "Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter effect". European Journal of Social Psychology. 15 (3): 353–361. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420150309.
  • Nuttin, Jozef M. Jr. (1987). "Affective consequences of mere ownership: The name letter effect in twelve European languages". European Journal of Social Psychology. 17 (4): 381–402. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420170402.
  • Pelham, B.; Mirenberg, Matthew C.; Jones, John T. (2002). "Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (4): 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.469. PMID 11999918.
  • Pelham, Brett; Carvallo, Mauricio (2015). "When Tex and Tess Carpenter build houses in Texas: Moderators of implicit egotism". Self and Identity. 4 (6): 692–723. doi:10.1080/15298868.2015.1070745. S2CID 145572523.
  • Polman, Evan; Pollmann, Monique M. H.; Poehlman, T. Andrew (2013). "The name-letter-effect in groups: Sharing initials with group members increases the quality of group work". PLoS ONE. 8 (11): e79039. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...879039P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079039. PMC 3827308. PMID 24236087.
  • Sas, Hilde (1986). Generality of Nuttin's name letter effect and asymmetrical letter choice effect: Some preliminary results of a cross-sectional and cross-lingual study. Leuven, Belgium: Laboratorium voor Experimentele Sociale Psychology, K.U. Leuven.
  • Simonsohn, Uri (2011a). "Spurious? Name similarity effects (implicit egotism) in marriage, job, and moving decisions". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 101 (1): 1–24. doi:10.1037/a0021990. PMID 21299311. S2CID 207744109.
  • Simonsohn, Uri (2011b). "Spurious also? Name-similarity effects (implicit egotism) in employment decisions". Psychological Science. 22 (8): 1087–1089. doi:10.1177/0956797611413937. PMID 21705520. S2CID 9821222.
  • Smith, Gary (2011). "Another look at baseball player initials and longevity". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 112 (1): 211–216. doi:10.2466/05.PMS.112.1.211-216. PMID 21466094. S2CID 37168549.
  • Spalding, Leah R.; Hardin, Curtis D. (1999). "Unconscious unease and self-handicapping: Behavioral consequences of individual differences in implicit and explicit self-esteem". Psychological Science. 10 (6): 535–539. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00202. S2CID 146547054.
  • Stieger, Stefan (2010). "Name-letter branding under scrutiny: Real products, new algorithms, and the probability of buying". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 110 (3, Pt 2): 1089–1097. doi:10.2466/01.07.PMS.110.C.1089-1097. PMID 20865997.
  • Stieger, S.; LeBel, E. (2012). "Name–letter preferences for new last name and abandoned birth name initials in the context of name-change via marriage". Social Psychology. 43 (1): 7–13. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000075.
  • Stieger, S.; Voracek, M.; Formann, A.K. (2012). "How to administer the Initial Preference Task". European Journal of Personality. 26: 63–78. doi:10.1002/per.823. S2CID 144719071.
  • Stieger, S.; Preyss, A.V.; Voracek, M. (2012). "Romantic jealousy and implicit and explicit self-esteem". Personality and Individual Differences. 52: 51–55. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.028.
  • Stieger, S.; Burger, C.; Schiller, F.R.; Schulze, E.K.; Voracek, M. (2014). "Measuring implicit gender-role orientation: The Gender Initial Preference Task". Journal of Personality Assessment. 96 (3): 358–367. doi:10.1080/00223891.2013.825622. PMID 23980799. S2CID 10514559.
  • Syz, Hans C. (1926). "Observations on the unreliability of subjective reports of emotional reactions". British Journal of Psychology. 17 (2): 119–126. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1926.tb00415.x. ISSN 0007-1269.
  • Treiman, Rebecca; Broderick, Victor (1998). "What's in a name: Children's knowledge about the letters in their own name". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 70 (2): 97–116. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2448. PMID 9729451.
  • Voracek, Martin; Rieder, Stephan; Stieger, Stefan; Swami, Viren (2015). "What's in a surname? Physique, aptitude, and sports type comparisons between Tailors and Smiths". PLOS ONE. 10 (7): 699–702. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1031795V. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0131795. PMC 4498760. PMID 26161803.
  • Wentura, Dirk; Kulfanek, Michael; Greve, Werner (2001). "Masked affective priming by name letters: Evidence for a correspondence of explicit and implicit self-esteem". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 41 (6): 657–663. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.005.
  • Wiebenga, Jacob H.; Fennis, Bob M. (2012). "Me, myself, and Ikea: Qualifying the role of implicit egotism in brand judgment" (PDF). In Gürhan-Canli, Zeynep; Otnes, Cele; Zhu, Rui (eds.). NA - Advances in Consumer Research. 40. Duluth, Minnesota: Association for Consumer Research. pp. 733–734. ISBN 978-0-915552-70-2.
  • Wilson, T. D.; Dunn, D. S.; Kraft, D.; Lisle, D. J. (1989). "Introspection, attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: The disruptive effects of explaining why we feel the way we do". In Berkowitz, L. (ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology. 22. Orlando, Florida: Academic Press. pp. 287–343.
  • Zajonc, Robert B. (1968). "Attitudinal effects of mere exposure". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 9 (2, Pt.2): 1–27. doi:10.1037/h0025848.
  • Zajonc, Robert B. (1980). "Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences". American Psychologist. 35 (2, Pt.2): 151–175. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Name-letter_effect&oldid=1225491307"

    Categories: 
    Cognitive biases
    Human names
    Alphabets
    1985 in science
    Hidden categories: 
    Articles with short description
    Short description is different from Wikidata
    Featured articles
    Use American English from May 2023
    All Wikipedia articles written in American English
    Use mdy dates from May 2023
    Pages containing links to subscription-only content
     



    This page was last edited on 24 May 2024, at 19:15 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki