Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Possible explanations  





2 See also  





3 References  














Numerosity adaptation effect






فارسی
Magyar
 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 




In other projects  



Wikimedia Commons
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Two figures, one above the other. First figure (up) display contain two circles: 10 dots in the circle at left and 100 dots in the circle at right. Fixation mark is placed between two circles. The second figure display contain two circles with each of them contain 40 dots. The dots in the left circle are arranged as mirror image of the dots in the right circle
An example of the numerosity adaptation effect

The numerosity adaptation effect is a perceptual phenomenoninnumerical cognition which demonstrates non-symbolic numerical intuition and exemplifies how numerical percepts can impose themselves upon the human brain automatically. This effect was first described in 2008.[1][2]

Presently, this effect is described only for controlled experimental conditions. In the illustration, a viewer should have a strong impression that the left display (lower figure) is more numerous than the right, after 30 seconds of viewing the adaptation (upper figure), although both have exactly the same number of dots. The viewer might also underestimate the number of dots presented in the display.[1]

Both effects are resistant to manipulation of the non-numerical parameters of the display. Thus, this effect cannot be simply explained in terms of size, density, or contrast.[3][4]

Perhaps the most astonishing aspect of these effects is that they happen immediately, and without conscious control (i.e., knowing that the numbers are equal would not hamper their happening).[1] This points to the operation of a special and largely automatic processing system. As noted by Burr & Ross (2008):

Just as we have a direct visual sense of the reddishness of half a dozen ripe cherries, so we do of their sixishness.[2]

Possible explanations[edit]

Few explanations were suggested to explain these phenomena. It was argued that they are heavily dependent on density and less on numerosity. Also, it was suggested that numerosity may be correlated with kurtosis and that the results may be better explained in terms of texture density such that only dots falling within the spatial region where the test is displayed effectively adapt the region.[5]

However, as the display in the original experiments was of spots uniformly either white or black, the kurtosis account is inapplicable. The texture density explanation doesn't seem to disentangle the complexity of these phenomena as in the display the left field adapts to many dots, the right field to few, and these adapters selectively affect the relevant test stimuli. It is not the number of dots in the entire display that causes the adaptation but only those within a particular area.[4] At present, why adaptation have such profound effect on numerosity estimates remains largely unexplained.[1]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c d Dehaene, Stanislas (2009). "Origins of Mathematical Intuitions The Case of Arithmetic". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156: 232–259. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04469.x. PMID 19338511. S2CID 2382715.
  • ^ a b Burr, David; John Ross (2008). "A Visual Sense of Number". Current Biology. 18 (6): 425–428. Bibcode:2008CBio...18..425B. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.052. PMID 18342507.
  • ^ Izard, Véronique; Stanislas Dehaene (2008). "Calibrating the mental number line" (PDF). Cognition. 106 (3): 1221–1247. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.148.2960. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.004. PMID 17678639. S2CID 9488163. Retrieved 2010-04-01.
  • ^ a b Burr, David; John Ross (September 23, 2008). "Response: Visual number". Current Biology. 18 (18): R857–R858. Bibcode:2008CBio...18.R857B. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.052.
  • ^ Durgin, Frank H (September 23, 2008). "Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited". Current Biology. 18 (18): R855–R856. Bibcode:2008CBio...18.R855D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.053. PMID 18812077.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Numerosity_adaptation_effect&oldid=1214467917"

    Categories: 
    Optical illusions
    Cognition
    Perception
    Hidden categories: 
    Articles with short description
    Short description matches Wikidata
    Commons link is locally defined
     



    This page was last edited on 19 March 2024, at 03:46 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki