Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Background  



1.1  Philosophy  





1.2  Scientific investigation  



1.2.1  Mammals  





1.2.2  Birds  





1.2.3  Reptiles  





1.2.4  Fish  





1.2.5  Argument by analogy  







1.3  Notes  







2 Experiencing pain  



2.1  Nociception  





2.2  Emotional pain  





2.3  Physical pain  







3 Adaptive value  





4 Research findings  



4.1  Nervous system  



4.1.1  Receptors  





4.1.2  Nerve fibres  





4.1.3  Brain  







4.2  Opioid system and effects of analgesics  



4.2.1  Effects of morphine and other opioids  





4.2.2  Effects of opioid antagonists  





4.2.3  Effects of other analgesics  







4.3  Physiological changes  





4.4  Protective motor responses  





4.5  Avoidance learning  





4.6  Trade-offs in motivation  





4.7  Cognitive ability and sentience  



4.7.1  Habituation  





4.7.2  Associative learning  





4.7.3  Numeracy  





4.7.4  Spatial orientation  





4.7.5  Social learning  









5 Criteria for pain perception  





6 Scientific statements  





7 Societal implications  



7.1  Culinary  





7.2  Legislation  







8 See also  





9 References  














Pain in amphibians






فارسی
 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Dissection of a frog

Pain is an aversive sensation and feeling associated with actual, or potential, tissue damage.[1] It is widely accepted by a broad spectrum of scientists and philosophers that non-human animals can perceive pain, including pain in amphibians.

Pain is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with suffering, which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of pain in non-human animals cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of phenomenal consciousness which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.[2][3]

Amphibians, particularly anurans, fulfill several physiological and behavioural criteria proposed as indicating that non-human animals may experience pain. These fulfilled criteria include a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors, opioid receptors and reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics, physiological changes to noxious stimuli, displaying protective motor reactions, exhibiting avoidance learning and making trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements.

Pain in amphibians has societal implications including their exposure to pollutants, (preparation for) cuisine (e.g. frog legs) and amphibians used in scientific research.

Several scientists and scientific groups have expressed the belief that amphibians can feel pain, however, this remains somewhat controversial due to differences in brain structure and the nervous system compared with other vertebrates.

Examples of the three modern orders of amphibians

Background

[edit]

The possibility that amphibians and other non-human animals may experience pain has a long history. Initially, pain in non-human animals was based around theoretical and philosophical argument, but more recently has turned to scientific investigation.

Philosophy

[edit]
René Descartes

The idea that non-human animals might not feel pain goes back to the 17th-century French philosopher, René Descartes, who argued that animals do not experience pain and suffering because they lack consciousness.[4][5][6] In 1789, the British philosopher and social reformist, Jeremy Bentham, addressed in his book An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation the issue of our treatment of animals with the following often quoted words: "The question is not, Can they reason? nor, can they talk? but, Can they suffer?"[7]

Peter Singer, a bioethicist and author of Animal Liberation published in 1975, suggested that consciousness is not necessarily the key issue: just because animals have smaller brains, or are ‘less conscious’, this does not mean that they are not capable of feeling pain.

Bernard Rollin, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, writes that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain.[8] In his interactions with scientists and other veterinarians, Rollin was regularly asked to "prove" that animals are conscious, and to provide "scientifically acceptable" grounds for claiming that they feel pain.[8]

Continuing into the 1990s, discussions were further developed on the roles that philosophy and science had in understanding animal cognition and mentality.[9] In subsequent years, it was argued there was strong support for the suggestion that some animals (most likely amniotes) have at least simple conscious thoughts and feelings[10] and that the view animals feel pain differently to higher primates is now a minority view.[4]

Scientific investigation

[edit]
Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness (2012)

The absence of a neocortex does not appear to preclude an organism from experiencing affective states. Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors. Consequently, the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.[11]

In the 20th- and 21st-century, there were many scientific investigations of pain in non-human animals.

Mammals

[edit]

At the turn of the century, studies were published showing that arthritic rats self-select analgesic opiates.[12] In 2014, the veterinary Journal of Small Animal Practice published an article on the recognition of pain which started "The ability to experience pain is universally shared by all mammals...",[13] and in 2015, it was reported in the science journal Pain that several mammalian species (rat, mouse, rabbit, cat and horse) adopt a facial expression in response to a noxious stimulus that is consistent with the expression of pain.[14]

Birds

[edit]

At the same time as the investigations using arthritic rats, studies were published showing that birds with gait abnormalities self-select for a diet that contains carprofen, an analgesic.[15] In 2005, it was written "Avian pain is likely analogous to pain experienced by most mammals"[16] and in 2014, "...it is accepted that birds perceive and respond to noxious stimuli and that birds feel pain."[17]

Reptiles

[edit]

Veterinary articles have been published stating reptiles[18][19][20] experience pain in a way analogous to mammals, and that analgesics are effective in this class of vertebrates.

Fish

[edit]

Several scientists or scientific groups have made statements indicating they believe fish can experience pain. For example, in 2004, Chandroo et al. wrote "Anatomical, pharmacological and behavioural data suggest that affective states of pain, fear and stress are likely to be experienced by fish in similar ways as in tetrapods".[21] In 2009, the European Food Safety Authority published a document stating scientific opinion on the welfare of fish. The document contains many sections indicating that the scientific panel believe fish can experience pain, for example, "Fish that are simply immobilized or paralysed [before euthanasia] would experience pain and suffering..."[22] In 2015, Brown wrote "A review of the evidence for pain perception strongly suggests that fish experience pain in a manner similar to the rest of the vertebrates."[23]

Argument by analogy

[edit]

In 2012 the American philosopher Gary Varner reviewed the research literature on pain in animals. His findings are summarised in the following table.[24]

Argument by analogy[24]

Property

Fish

Amphibians

Reptiles

Birds

Mammals

Has nociceptors

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Has brain

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Nociceptors and brain linked

Green tickY

?[a] / Green tickY

?[b] / Green tickY

? / Green tickY

Green tickY

Has endogenous opioids

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Analgesics affect responses

Green tickY

?[c]

?[d]

Green tickY

Green tickY

Response to damaging stimuli similar to humans

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Green tickY

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ But see[25]
  • ^ But see[26]
  • ^ But see[27]
  • ^ But see[28]
  • Arguing by analogy, Varner claims that any animal which exhibits the properties listed in the table could be said to experience pain. On that basis, he concludes that all vertebrates, including amphibians, probably experience pain, but invertebrates apart from cephalopods probably do not experience pain.[24][29]

    Experiencing pain

    [edit]

    Although there are numerous definitions of pain, almost all involve two key components.

    First, nociception is required.[30] This is the ability to detect noxious stimuli which evoke a reflex response that rapidly moves the entire animal, or the affected part of its body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective "feeling" – it is a reflex action. An example would be the rapid withdrawal of a finger that has touched something hot – the withdrawal occurs before any sensation of pain is actually experienced.

    The second component is the experience of "pain" itself, or suffering – the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience. This is when the withdrawn finger begins to hurt, moments after the withdrawal. Pain is therefore a private, emotional experience. Pain cannot be directly measured in other animals; responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, argument-by-analogy is used. This is based on the principle that if an animal responds to a stimulus in a similar way, it is likely to have had an analogous experience.

    Nociception

    [edit]
    Nociception: The reflex arc of a dog with a pin in her paw. Note there is no communication to the brain, but the paw is withdrawn by nervous impulses generated by the spinal cord. There is no conscious interpretation of the stimulus by the dog.

    Nociception usually involves the transmission of a signal along a chain of nerve fibers from the site of a noxious stimulus at the periphery to the spinal cord and brain. This process evokes a reflex arc response generated at the spinal cord and not involving the brain, such as flinching or withdrawal of a limb. Nociception is found, in one form or another, across all major animal taxa.[30] Nociception can be observed using modern imaging techniques; and a physiological and behavioral response to nociception can be detected.

    Emotional pain

    [edit]

    Sometimes a distinction is made between "physical pain" and "emotional" or "psychological pain". Emotional pain is the pain experienced in the absence of physical trauma, e.g. the pain experienced after the loss of a loved one, or the break-up of a relationship. It has been argued that only primates can feel "emotional pain", because they are the only animals that have a neocortex – a part of the brain's cortex considered to be the "thinking area". However, research has provided evidence that monkeys, dogs, cats and birds can show signs of emotional pain and display behaviours associated with depression during painful experience, i.e. lack of motivation, lethargy, anorexia, unresponsiveness to other animals.[31]

    Physical pain

    [edit]

    The nerve impulses of the nociception response may be conducted to the brain thereby registering the location, intensity, quality and unpleasantness of the stimulus. This subjective component of pain involves conscious awareness of both the sensation and the unpleasantness (the aversive, negative affect). The brain processes underlying conscious awareness of the unpleasantness (suffering), are not well understood.

    There have been several published lists of criteria for establishing whether non-human animals experience pain, e.g.[32][33] Some criteria that may indicate the potential of another species, including amphibians, to feel pain include:[33]

    1. Has a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors
    2. Has opioid receptors and shows reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics
    3. Physiological changes to noxious stimuli
    4. Displays protective motor reactions that might include reduced use of an affected area such as limping, rubbing, holding or autotomy
    5. Shows avoidance learning
    6. Shows trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements
    7. High cognitive ability and sentience

    Adaptive value

    [edit]

    The adaptive value of nociception is obvious; an organism detecting a noxious stimulus immediately withdraws the limb, appendage or entire body from the noxious stimulus and thereby avoids further (potential) injury. However, a characteristic of pain (in mammals at least) is that pain can result in hyperalgesia (a heightened sensitivity to noxious stimuli) and allodynia (a heightened sensitivity to non-noxious stimuli). When this heightened sensitisation occurs, the adaptive value is less clear. First, the pain arising from the heightened sensitisation can be disproportionate to the actual tissue damage caused. Second, the heightened sensitisation may also become chronic, persisting well beyond the tissues healing. This can mean that rather than the actual tissue damage causing pain, it is the pain due to the heightened sensitisation that becomes the concern. This means the sensitisation process is sometimes termed maladaptive. It is often suggested hyperalgesia and allodynia assist organisms to protect themselves during healing, but experimental evidence to support this has been lacking.[34][35]

    In 2014, the adaptive value of sensitisation due to injury was tested using the predatory interactions between longfin inshore squid (Doryteuthis pealeii) and black sea bass (Centropristis striata) which are natural predators of this squid. If injured squid are targeted by a bass, they began their defensive behaviours sooner (indicated by greater alert distances and longer flight initiation distances) than uninjured squid. If anaesthetic (1% ethanol and MgCl2) is administered prior to the injury, this prevents the sensitisation and blocks the behavioural effect. The authors claim this study is the first experimental evidence to support the argument that nociceptive sensitisation is actually an adaptive response to injuries.[36]

    Research findings

    [edit]

    Nervous system

    [edit]

    Receptors

    [edit]

    Frogs have nociceptors in the superficial and deep layers of the skin that transduce mechanical and chemical noxious stimuli. Furthermore, frogs possess neural pathways that support processing and perception of noxious stimuli. Although organization is less well structured compared with mammals, it is now commonly accepted that amphibians possess neuro-anatomical pathways conductive of a complete nociceptive experience.[25]

    Nerve fibres

    [edit]

    Early electrophysiological studies in frogs report that noxious mechanical, thermal and chemical stimuli excite primary afferent fibres with slowly conducting axons.[37]

    There are two types of nerve fibre relevant to pain in amphibians. Group C nerve fibres are a type of sensory nerve fibre which lack a myelin sheath and have a small diameter, meaning they have a low nerve conduction velocity. The suffering associated with burns, toothaches, or crushing injury are caused by C fibre activity. A-delta fibres are another type of sensory nerve fibre, however, these are myelinated and therefore transmit impulses faster than non-myelinated C fibres. A-delta fibres carry cold, pressure and some pain signals, and are associated with acute pain that results in "pulling away" from noxious stimuli.[38]

    The skin of frogs contains both Group C fibres and A-delta fibres.[25][37]

    Brain

    [edit]
    Brains of vertebrate classes. CB., cerebellum; PT., pituitary body; PN., pineal body; C. STR., corpus striatum; G.H.R., right ganglion habenulæ. I., olfactory; II., optic nerves.

    All vertebrate species have a common brain archetype divided into the telencephalon and diencephalon (collectively referred to as forebrain), mesencephalon (midbrain) and rhombencephalon (hindbrain).[39] Nervous connections to the telencephalon indicate that frogs may be able to perceive pain.[25]

    In 2002, James Rose, from the University of Wyoming, published reviews arguing that fish cannot feel pain because they lack a neocortex in the brain.[40][41] If the presence of a large, considerably developed neocortex is required for experiencing pain, as Rose suggests, this theory would eliminate birds, amphibians, other non-mammalian animals, and even some mammals from having the capacity to experience pain.[42] Other researchers do not believe that animal consciousness requires a neocortex, but can arise from homologous subcortical brain networks.[11] Animal behaviouralist Temple Grandin argues that fish (and therefore, presumably, amphibians) could still have consciousness without a neocortex because "different species can use different brain structures and systems to handle the same functions."[43]

    Opioid system and effects of analgesics

    [edit]

    By spinal administration of a range of opioid agonists, it has been demonstrated that frogs have mu (μ)-, delta (δ) and kappa (κ)-opioid binding sites.[44] The kappa sub-types κ1 and κ2 are present in the brains of edible frogs (Rana esculenta). In evolutionary terms, this means the opioid receptor sub-types are already present in amphibians, although the differences between these are less pronounced than in mammals.[45] Sequence comparisons show that the amphibian opioid receptors are highly conserved (70-84% similar to mammals) and are expressed in the central nervous system (CNS) areas apparently involved in pain experience.[32]

    When treating amphibians, veterinary practice frequently uses the same analgesics and anesthetics used for mammals. These chemicals act on the nociceptive pathways, blocking signals to the brain where emotional responses to the signals are further processed by certain parts of the brain found in amniotes ("higher vertebrates").[46][47]

    Effects of morphine and other opioids

    [edit]

    The relative analgesic potency of 11 opioid agents (μ-opioid receptor agonists – fentanyl, levorphanol, methadone, morphine, meperidine and codeine; the partial μ agonist – buprenorphine; and the κ-opioid receptor agonists – nalorphine, bremazocine, U50488 and CI-977) in the Northern grass frog produced a dose-dependent and long-lasting analgesia which persists for at least four hours. The relative analgesic potency of μ-opioids in amphibians was correlated with the relative analgesic potency of these same agents recorded in on the mouse writhing and hot plate tests.[48][49] Other opioid analgesics are effective in amphibians, for example, butorphanol.[50]

    Alfaxalonebutorphanol and alfaxalone–morphine combinations are comparable in terms of onset and duration of anaesthesia in Oriental fire-bellied toads (Bombina orientalis).[51]

    When an isolated peptide termed "frog's nociception-related peptide" (fNRP) is injected into newts, it increases the latency for newts to flick their tails in response to a hot-beam. The effect is blocked by simultaneous injection of naloxone, thereby indicating evidence for the interaction of fNRP and opioid steps in the analgesia pathways of newts.[52]

    Effects of opioid antagonists

    [edit]

    Naloxone and naltrexone are both μ-opioid receptor antagonists which, in mammals, negate the analgesic effects of opioids. Morphine analgesia in frogs is blocked by both naloxone and naltrexone, indicating that the effect is mediated at least partially by opioid receptors.[53]

    Effects of other analgesics

    [edit]

    Direct intraspinal injection of the catecholamines epinephrine and norepinephrine, and the α-adrenergic agents dexmedetomidine and clonidine, produce a dose-dependent elevation of pain thresholds in the Northern leopard frog (Rana pipiens). This analgesia occurs without accompanying motor or sedative effects.[54]

    A range of non-opioid drugs administered through the dorsal lymph sac of Northern leopard frogs has demonstrable analgesic effects, established by using the acetic acid test. Chlorpromazine and haloperidol (antipsychotics), chlordiazepoxide (a benzodiazepine) and diphenhydramine (a histamine antagonist) produced moderate to strong analgesic effects, whereas indomethacin and ketorolac (NSAIDs), and pentobarbital (a barbiturate) produced weaker analgesic effects.[55]

    Physiological changes

    [edit]

    In multiple animal studies, it has been shown that stress causes increases in glucocorticoid levels).[56] Frogs release corticosteroids in response to many environmental factors[57] and this pattern of release is often species-specific within Amphibia[58] More specifically, increased stocking density and hypoxia cause changes in cortisol (one of the glucocorticoids) and white blood cells in American bullfrog tadpoles (Lithobates catesbeianus) indicative of stress.[58]

    Analgesia in amphibians can be measured using heart rate and respiratory rate.[51]

    Protective motor responses

    [edit]

    Amphibians exhibit classic wiping and withdrawal protective motor responses to noxious chemical, heat and mechanical stimuli.[32]

    Acetic acid (a strong irritant) applied to the hindlimb of frogs elicits vigorous wiping of the exposed skin; both pH and osmolarity may contribute to the nociception produced.[59] This response is used in a standard test for analgesic effects in frogs, commonly termed the "acetic acid test". In this procedure, dilutions of the acid are placed drop-wise on the dorsum of the frog's thigh until the frog wipes the affected area.[55]

    Newts flick their tails in response to it being irradiated by a hot beam,[52] in a very similar manner to that observed in rodents being used in the tail flick test.

    The threshold to Von Frey hairs and response to nociceptive withdrawal can be used to measure the effectiveness of analgesia.[51]

    Avoidance learning

    [edit]

    Early studies showed that African clawed frogs (Xenopus laevis) learn to avoid electric shocks in an aquatic shuttle-box test[60] and similarly, cane toads (Bufo marinus) learn to avoid electric shocks in a T-maze.[61] Furthermore, American bullfrogs (Rana catesbiana) learn to inhibit their high-priority, biologically adaptive righting reflex to avoid electric shock; after training, they remain passively on their backs rather than exhibiting the normal short-latency, righting response.[62]

    Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis is a chytrid fungus that causes the disease chytridiomycosis in amphibians; frogs learn to avoid the fungus after just one exposure.[63]

    Trade-offs in motivation

    [edit]

    A painful experience may change the motivation for normal behavioural responses. American bullfrogs learn to inhibit their high-priority, biologically adaptive righting reflex to avoid electric shock. After repeated exposure, they remain passively on their backs rather than exhibiting the normal, short-latency, righting response,[62] thereby showing a trade-off in motivation.

    Cognitive ability and sentience

    [edit]
    Giant salamander

    It has been argued that although a high cognitive capacity may indicate a greater likelihood of experiencing pain, it also gives these animals a greater ability to deal with this, leaving animals with a lower cognitive ability a greater problem in coping with pain.[64]

    Habituation

    [edit]

    Habituation is one of the simplest forms of animal learning. It has been stated there are no qualitative or quantitative differences between vertebrate species in this form of learning[65] indicating there is no difference between mammals and amphibians in this process.

    Associative learning

    [edit]

    Newts are capable of associative learning. They are able to associate chemical signals from a novel predator with another chemical stimulus when the second stimulus is the skin extract of another newt.[66]

    Numeracy

    [edit]

    At least some amphibians are capable of numeracy.[67][68] When offered live fruit flies (Drosophila virilis), salamanders choose the larger of 1 vs 2 and 2 vs 3. Frogs are able to distinguish between low numbers (1 vs 2, 2 vs 3, but not 3 vs 4) and large numbers (3 vs 6, 4 vs 8, but not 4 vs 6) of prey. This is irrespective of other characteristics, i.e. surface area, volume, weight and movement, although discrimination among large numbers may be based on surface area.[69]

    Spatial orientation

    [edit]
    The spotted salamander can learn to use visual cues to locate rewards.

    The Rocky Mountain toad (Bufo woodhousii woodhousii) and Gulf Coast toad (Bufo valliceps) are able to discriminate between left and right positions in a T-maze.[70]

    Both the terrestrial toad Rhinella arenarum[71] and the spotted salamander (Ambystoma maculatum)[72] can learn to orient in an open space using visual cues to get to a reward. Furthermore, they prefer using cues close to the reward. This shows a learning phenomenon previously recorded in other taxa including mammals, birds, fish and invertebrates.[71] It has been suggested that male dart frogs of the species Allobates femoralis use spatial learning for way-finding in their local area; they are able to find their way back to their territory when displaced several hundred metres, so long as they are displaced in their local area.[73]

    Social learning

    [edit]

    Wood frog (Rana sylvatica) tadpoles use social learning to acquire information about predators; the ratio of tutors to observers, but not group size, influences the intensity of learned predator recognition.[74] Wood frog tadpoles also exhibit local enhancement in their social learning, however, spotted salamander larvae do not; this difference in social learning could be largely due to differences in aquatic ecology between tadpoles and salamander larvae.[75]

    Criteria for pain perception

    [edit]

    Scientists have also proposed that in conjunction with argument-by-analogy, criteria of physiology or behavioural responses can be used to assess the possibility of non-human animals perceiving pain. The following is a table of criteria suggested by Sneddon et al.[32]

    Criteria for pain perception in amphibians

    Criteria

    Anura

    Caudata

    Gymnophiona

    Has nociceptors

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Pathways to central nervous system

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Central processing in brain

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Receptors for analgesic drugs

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Physiological responses

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Movement away from noxious stimuli

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Behavioural changes from norm

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Protective behaviour

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Responses reduced by analgesic drugs

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Self-administration of analgesia

    ?

    ?

    ?

    Responses with high priority over other stimuli

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Pay cost to access analgesia

    ?

    ?

    ?

    Altered behavioural choices/preferences

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Relief learning

    ?

    ?

    ?

    Rubbing, limping or guarding

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Paying a cost to avoid noxious stimulus

    ?

    ?

    ?

    Tradeoffs with other requirements

    Green tickY

    ?

    ?

    Scientific statements

    [edit]

    Several scientists have made statements indicating they believe amphibians can experience pain. For example, -

    After examining the morphology of the nervous system of vertebrates, Somme concluded "...most four-legged vertebrates have some state of consciousness..."[76]

    Gentz, in a paper on the surgery of amphibians, writes "Postoperative recommendations include ...analgesia" and "Hypothermia is also unacceptable as a sedation technique for painful procedures".[50]

    Veterinary articles have been published stating amphibians experience pain in a way analogous to mammals, and that analgesics are effective in control of this class of vertebrates.[77][78][79] Shine et al., wrote that most animal ethics committees and the wider community believe that amphibians can feel pain.[80]

    Some scientists have been a little more guarded about the experience of amphibians, for example, Michaels et al. wrote that the identification of pain pathways shared between amphibians and other amniotes suggests an ability to experience pain, even if in a different and more restricted sense than in amniote taxa.[81]

    Societal implications

    [edit]

    Societal implications of pain in amphibians include acute and chronic exposure to pollutants, cuisine and scientific research (e.g. genetic-modification may have detrimental effects on welfare, deliberately-imposed adverse physical, physiological and behavioural states, toe-clipping or other methods of invasive marking and handling procedures which may cause injury).

    Culinary

    [edit]
    Frogs legs – a culinary dish

    It has been claimed that frogs killed for eating are "...sliced through the belly while they are still fully conscious and they can take up to an hour to die."[82]

    Legislation

    [edit]

    In the UK, the legislation protecting animals during scientific research, the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986, protects amphibians from the moment they become capable of independent feeding.[83] The legislation protecting animals in most other circumstances in the UK is the Animal Welfare Act 2006, which states that in the Act, "'animal means a vertebrate other than man",[84] thereby including amphibians.

    The 1974 Norwegian Animal Rights Law states it relates to mammals, birds, frogs, salamanders, reptiles, fish, and crustaceans.[85]

    In the US, the legislation protecting animals during scientific research is the Animal Welfare Act.[86] This Act excludes protection of "cold-blooded" animals, thereby excluding amphibians from protection.

    See also

    [edit]
  • Animal consciousness
  • Animal cruelty
  • Ethics of eating meat
  • Ethics of uncertain sentience
  • Moral status of animals in the ancient world
  • Pain and suffering in laboratory animals
  • Sentience
  • N-Acylethanolamine
  • References

    [edit]
    1. ^ Broom, D.M. (2001). "Evolution of pain" (PDF). Vlaams Diergeneeskundig Tijdschrift. 70 (1): 17–21. doi:10.21825/vdt.89895.
  • ^ Abbott, F.V., Franklin, K.B.J. and Westbrook, R.F. (1995). "The formalin test: Scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats". Pain. 60 (1): 91–102. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V. PMID 7715946. S2CID 35448280.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Key, B. (2015). "Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness". Biology and Philosophy. 30 (2): 149–165. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4. PMC 4356734. PMID 25798021.
  • ^ a b Carbone, L. (2004). What Animals Want: Expertise and Advocacy in Laboratory Animal Welfare Policy. Oxford University Press. p. 149. ISBN 9780195161960.
  • ^ Radner, D. & Radner, M. (1989). Animal Consciousness. Prometheus Books: Buffalo.
  • ^ Harrison, P. (1992). "Descartes on animals". The Philosophical Quarterly. 42 (167): 219–227. doi:10.2307/2220217. JSTOR 2220217.
  • ^ "Bentham, J. (1879). An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. Clarendon Press.
  • ^ a b Rollin, B. (1989). The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. Oxford University Press, pp. xii, 117-118, cited in Carbone 2004, p. 150.
  • ^ Allen, C. (1998). "Assessing animal cognition: Ethological and philosophical perspectives". Journal of Animal Science. 76 (1): 42–47. doi:10.2527/1998.76142x. PMID 9464883.
  • ^ Griffin, D.R. & Speck, G.B. (2004). "New evidence of animal consciousness". Animal Cognition. 7 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x. PMID 14658059. S2CID 8650837.
  • ^ a b Low, P. (July 7, 2012). Jaak Panksepp; Diana Reiss; David Edelman; Bruno Van Swinderen; Philip Low; Christof Koch (eds.). "The Cambridge declaration on consciousness" (PDF). University of Cambridge.
  • ^ Colpaert, F.C., Tarayre, J.P., Alliaga, M., Slot. L.A.B., Attal, N. and Koek, W. (2001). "Opiate self-administration as a measure of chronic nociceptive pain in arthritic rats". Pain. 91 (1–2): 33–45. doi:10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00413-9. PMID 11240076. S2CID 24858615.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Mathews, K., Kronen, P.W., Lascelles, D., Nolan, A., Robertson, S., Steagall, P.V., Wright, B. and Yamashita, K. (2014). "Guidelines for recognition, assessment and treatment of pain". Journal of Small Animal Practice. 55 (6): E10–E68. doi:10.1111/jsap.12200. PMID 24841489.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Chambers, C.T. and Mogil, J.S. (2015). "Ontogeny and phylogeny of facial expression of pain". Pain. 156 (5): 798–799. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000133. PMID 25887392. S2CID 2060896.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Danbury, T.C., Weeks, C.A., Chambers, J.P., Waterman-Pearson, A.E. and Kestin, S.C. (2000). "Self-selection of the analgesic drug carprofen by lame broiler chickens". The Veterinary Record. 146 (11): 307–311. doi:10.1136/vr.146.11.307. PMID 10766114. S2CID 35062797.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Machin, K.L. (2005). "Avian analgesia". Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine. 14 (4): 236–242. doi:10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.004.
  • ^ Paul-Murphy, J. & Hawkins, M.G. (2014). "Chapter 26 - Bird-specific considerations: recognizing pain in pet birds.". In Gaynor, J.S. & Muir III, W. W. (eds.). Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management. Elsevier Health Sciences.
  • ^ Mosley, C.A. (2005). "Anesthesia & Analgesia in reptiles". Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine. 14 (4): 243–262. doi:10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.005.
  • ^ Mosley, C. (2011). "Pain and nociception in reptiles". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice. 14 (1): 45–60. doi:10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.009. PMID 21074702.
  • ^ Sladky, K.K. & Mans, C. (2012). "Clinical analgesia in reptiles". Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 21 (2): 158–167. doi:10.1053/j.jepm.2012.02.012.
  • ^ Chandroo, K.P., Duncan, I.J. and Moccia, R.D. (2004). "Can fish suffer?: perspectives on sentience, pain, fear and stress". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 86 (3): 225–250. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.327.8769. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2004.02.004.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Salman, J., Vannier, P. and Wierup. M. (2009). "Species-specific welfare aspects of the main systems of stunning and killing of farmed Atlantic salmon" (PDF). The EFSA Journal. 2012: 1–77.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Brown, C. (2015). "Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics". Animal Cognition. 18 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0. PMID 24942105. S2CID 207050888.
  • ^ a b c Varner, G.E. (2012). "Chapter 5 - Which animals are sentient? - the table in the article is based on Table 5.2, page 113". Personhood, Ethics, and Animal Cognition: Situating Animals in Hare's Two Level Utilitarianism. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199758784.001.0001. ISBN 9780199758784.
  • ^ a b c d Guénette, S.A., Giroux, M.C. and Vachon, P. (2013). "Pain perception and anaesthesia in research frogs". Experimental Animals. 62 (2): 87–92. doi:10.1538/expanim.62.87. PMID 23615302.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Mosley, C. (2006). "Pain, nociception and analgesia in reptiles: when your snake goes 'ouch!'" (PDF). The North American Veterinary Conference. 20: 1652–1653.
  • ^ Coble, D.J., Taylor, D.K. and Mook, D.M. (2011). "Analgesic effects of meloxicam, morphine sulfate, flunixin meglumine, and xylazine hydrochloride in African-clawed frogs (Xenopus laevis)". Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 50 (3): 355–60. PMC 3103286. PMID 21640031.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Baker, B.B., Sladky, K.K. and Johnson, S.M. (2011). "Evaluation of the analgesic effects of oral and subcutaneous tramadol administration in red-eared slider turtles". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 238 (2): 220–227. doi:10.2460/javma.238.2.220. PMC 3158493. PMID 21235376.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Andrews, K. (2014). The Animal Mind: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Animal Cognition - see section 3.6.2. Routledge. ISBN 9781317676751.
  • ^ a b Sneddon, L.U. (2004). "Evolution of nociception in vertebrates: comparative analysis of lower vertebrates". Brain Research Reviews. 46 (2): 123–130. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.07.007. PMID 15464201. S2CID 16056461.
  • ^ Sneddon, L.U. "Can animals feel pain?". The Welcome Trust. Archived from the original on April 13, 2012. Retrieved September 24, 2015.
  • ^ a b c d Sneddon, L.U., Elwood, R.W., Adamo, S.A. and Leach, M.C. (2014). "Defining and assessing animal pain". Animal Behaviour. 97: 201–212. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.007. S2CID 53194458.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ a b Elwood, R.W., Barr, S. and Patterson, L. (2009). "Pain and stress in crustaceans?". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 118 (3): 128–136. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Price, T.J. & Dussor, G. (2014). "Evolution: the advantage of 'maladaptive'pain plasticity". Current Biology. 24 (10): R384–R386. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.04.011. PMC 4295114. PMID 24845663.
  • ^ "Maladaptive pain". Oxford Reference. Retrieved May 16, 2016.
  • ^ Crook, R.J., Dickson, K., Hanlon, R.T. and Walters, E.T. (2014). "Nociceptive sensitization reduces predation risk". Current Biology. 24 (10): 1121–1125. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.043. PMID 24814149.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ a b Hamamoto, D. T. & Simone, D.A. (2003). "Characterization of cutaneous primary afferent fibers excited by acetic acid in a model of nociception in frogs". Journal of Neurophysiology. 90 (2): 566–577. doi:10.1152/jn.00324.2003. PMID 12750420. S2CID 15575676.
  • ^ Rose, J.D., Arlinghaus, R., Cooke, S.J., Diggles, B.K., Sawynok, W., Stevens, E.D. and Wynne, C.D.L. (2012). "Can fish really feel pain?" (PDF). Fish and Fisheries. 15 (1): 97–133. doi:10.1111/faf.12010.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Fabbro, F., Aglioti, S.M., Bergamasco, M., Clarici, A. and Panksepp, J. (2015). "Evolutionary aspects of self-and world consciousness in vertebrates". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 9: 157. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00157. PMC 4374625. PMID 25859205.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Rose, J.D. (2002). "The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain" (PDF). Reviews in Fisheries Science. 10 (1): 1–38. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.598.8119. doi:10.1080/20026491051668. S2CID 16220451. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-10-10.
  • ^ Rose, J.D. (2002). "Do fish feel pain?". Archived from the original on January 20, 2013. Retrieved September 27, 2007.
  • ^ Yue, S. (2008). "An HSUS report: fish and pain perception". Impacts on Farm Animals. Retrieved October 21, 2015.
  • ^ Grandin, T. & Johnson, C. (2005). Animals in Translation. New York: Scribner. pp. 183–184. ISBN 978-0-7432-4769-6.
  • ^ Stevens, C.W. (1996). "Relative analgesic potency of mu, delta and kappa opioids after spinal administration in amphibians". Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 276 (2): 440–448. PMID 8632308.
  • ^ Benyhe, S., Varga, E., Hepp, J., Magyar, A., Borsodi, A. and Wollemann, M. (1990). "Characterization of kappa1 and kappa2 opioid binding sites in frog (Rana esculenta) brain membrane preparation". Neurochemical Research. 15 (9): 899–904. doi:10.1007/bf00965909. PMID 2177154. S2CID 23867820.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Viñuela-Fernández I, Jones E, Welsh EM, Fleetwood-Walker SM (September 2007). "Pain mechanisms and their implication for the management of pain in farm and companion animals". Vet. J. 174 (2): 227–39. doi:10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.02.002. PMID 17553712.
  • ^ Sneddon, L.U. (2012). "Clinical Anesthesia & Analgesia in fish". Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 21: 32–43. doi:10.1053/j.jepm.2011.11.009. S2CID 78473.
  • ^ Stevens, C.W., Klopp, A.J. and Facello, J.A. (1994). "Analgesic potency of mu and kappa opioids after systemic administration in amphibians". Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 269 (3): 1086–1093. PMID 8014851.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Stevens, C.W., MacIver, D.N. and Newman, L.C. (2001). "Testing and comparison of non-opioid analgesics in amphibians". Contemporary Topics in Laboratory Animal Science. 40 (4): 23–7. PMC 3075466. PMID 11451391.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ a b Gentz E.J. (2007). "Medicine and surgery of amphibians". ILAR Journal. 48 (3): 255–259. doi:10.1093/ilar.48.3.255. PMID 17592187.
  • ^ a b c Adami, C., Spadavecchia, C., Angeli, G. and d'Ovidio, D. (2015). "Alfaxalone anesthesia by immersion in oriental fire-bellied toads (Bombina orientalis)". Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia. 42 (5): 547–551. doi:10.1111/vaa.12252. PMID 25711769.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ a b Kanetoh, T., Sugikawa, T., Sasaki, I., Muneoka, Y., Minakata, H., Takabatake, I. and Fujimoto, M. (2003). "Identification of a novel frog RFamide and its effect on the latency of the tail-flick response of the newt". Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology C. 134 (2): 259–266. doi:10.1016/s1532-0456(02)00277-6. PMID 12600686.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Suckow, M.A., Terril, L.A., Grigdesby, C.F. and March, P.A. (1999). "Evaluation of hypothermia-induced analgesia and influence of opioid antagonists in Leopard frogs (Rana pipiens)". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 63 (1): 39–43. doi:10.1016/s0091-3057(98)00237-8. PMID 10340522. S2CID 22059545.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Stevens, C.W. & Brenner, G.M. (1996). "Spinal administration of adrenergic agents produces analgesia in amphibians". European Journal of Pharmacology. 316 (2): 205–210. doi:10.1016/s0014-2999(96)00681-4. PMID 8982687.
  • ^ a b Stevens, C.W., MacIver, D.N. and Newman, L.C. (2001). "Testing and comparison of non-opioid analgesics in amphibians". Contemporary Topics in Laboratory Animal Science. 40 (4): 23–7. PMC 3075466. PMID 11451391.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Carlson, N.R. (2010). Physiology of Behavior, 11th Edition. New York: Allyn & Bacon. p. 605.
  • ^ Hanke, W. (2013) [1978]. "Chapter 5. The adrenal cortex of Amphibia". In I. Chester Jones; I.W. Henderson (eds.). General, Comparative and Clinical Endocrinology of the Adrenal Cortex, Volume 2. Academic Press. pp. 419–487.
  • ^ a b Teixeira, P.C., Dias, D.C., Rocha, G.C., Antonucci, A.M., França, F.M., Marcantonio, A.S., Ranzani-Paiva, M.T. and Ferreira, C.M. (2012). "Profile of cortisol, glycaemia, and blood parameters of American Bullfrog tadpoles Lithobates catesbeianus exposed to density and hypoxia stressors". Pesquisa Veterinária Brasileira. 32: 91–98. doi:10.1590/s0100-736x2012001300016. hdl:11449/574.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Hamamoto, D.T., Forkey, M.W., Davis, W.L., Kajander, K.C. and Simone, D.A. (2000). "The role of pH and osmolarity in evoking the acetic acid-induced wiping response in a model of nociception in frogs". Brain Research. 862 (1): 217–229. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02138-7. PMID 10799688. S2CID 7290178.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Miller, R.R., Berk, A.M. and Springer, A.D. (1974). "Acquisition and retention of active avoidance in Xenopus laevis". Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 3 (2): 139–141. doi:10.3758/BF03333423.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Williams Jr, J.T. (1967). "A test for dominance of cues during maze learning by toads". Psychonomic Science. 9 (5): 259–260. doi:10.3758/bf03332211. S2CID 143932516.
  • ^ a b Harvey, C.B., Ellis, C. and Tate, M. (1976). "Inhibition of the righting reflex in the common bullfrog (Rana catesbiana) employing an operant-avoidance procedure". Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 7 (1): 57–58. doi:10.3758/bf03337120.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ McMahon, T.A., Sears, B.F., Venesky, M.D., Bessler, S.M., Brown, J.M., Deutsch, K., ... and Rohr, J.R. (2014). "Amphibians acquire resistance to live and dead fungus overcoming fungal immunosuppression". Nature. 511 (7508): 224–227. Bibcode:2014Natur.511..224M. doi:10.1038/nature13491. PMC 4464781. PMID 25008531.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Broom, D.M. (2001). "Evolution of pain" (PDF). Vlaams Diergeneeskundig Tijdschrift. 70 (1): 17–21. doi:10.21825/vdt.89895.
  • ^ Lea, S.E.G. (1983). "Complex general learning in nonmammalian vertebrates.". In Marler, P.; Terrace, H.S (eds.). The Biology of Learning: Report of the Dahlem Workshop on the Biology of Learning, Berlin. Springer-Verlag. ISBN 9783642700941.
  • ^ Vitti, J. (2010). The Distribution and Evolution of Animal Consciousness (Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University)
  • ^ Krusche, P., Uller, C. and Dicke, U. (2010). "Quantity discrimination in salamanders". Journal of Experimental Biology. 213 (11): 1822–1828. doi:10.1242/jeb.039297. PMID 20472768.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Uller, C., Jaeger, R. and Guidry, G. (2003). "Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian". Animal Cognition. 6 (2): 105–112. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0167-x. PMID 12709845. S2CID 147018.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Stancher, G., Rugani, R., Regolin, L. and Vallortigara, G. (2015). "Numerical discrimination by frogs (Bombina orientalis)". Animal Cognition. 18 (1): 219–229. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0791-7. PMID 25108417. S2CID 16499583.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Chu, P.K. & McCain, G. (1969). "Discrimination learning and extinction in toads". Psychonomic Science. 14 (1): 14–15. doi:10.3758/bf03336400.
  • ^ a b Daneri, M.F., Casanave, E.B. and Muzio, R.N. (2015). "Use of local visual cues for spatial orientation in terrestrial toads (Rhinella arenarum): The role of distance to a goal". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 129 (3): 247–255. doi:10.1037/a0039461. hdl:11336/6327. PMID 26147701. S2CID 30988123.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Heuring, W. L. & Mathis, A. (2014). "Landmark learning by juvenile salamanders (Ambystoma maculatum)". Behavioural Processes. 108: 173–176. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.10.015. PMID 25444775. S2CID 45373288.
  • ^ Pašukonis, A., Warrington, I., Ringler, M. and Hödl, W. (2014). "Poison frogs rely on experience to find the way home in the rainforest". Biology Letters. 10 (11): 20140642. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0642. PMC 4261859. PMID 25411379.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Chivers, D.P. & Ferrari, M.C. (2015). "The effect of group size and tutor-to-observer ratio on socially learned antipredator responses in woodfrog tadpoles". Animal Behaviour. 104: 25–29. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.03.003. S2CID 53165376.
  • ^ Chapman, T.L., Holcomb, M.P., Spivey, K.L., Sehr, E.K. and Gall, B.G. (2015). "A test of local enhancement in amphibians". Ethology. 121 (3): 308–314. doi:10.1111/eth.12337.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Sømme, L.S. (2005). "Sentience and pain in invertebrates. Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety" (PDF). Norwegian University of Life Sciences. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  • ^ Machin, K.L. (1999). "Amphibian pain and analgesia". Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine. 30 (1): 2–10. JSTOR 20095815. PMID 10367638.
  • ^ Machin, K.L. (2001). "Fish, amphibian, and reptile analgesia". The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Exotic Animal Practice. 4 (1): 19–33. doi:10.1016/S1094-9194(17)30048-8. PMID 11217460.
  • ^ Stevens, C.W. (2011). "Analgesia in amphibians: preclinical studies and clinical applications". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice. 14 (1): 33–44. doi:10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.007. PMC 3056481. PMID 21074701.
  • ^ Shine, R., Amiel, J., Munn, A.J., Stewart, M., Vyssotski, A.L., and Lesku, J.A. (2015). "Is "cooling then freezing" a humane way to kill amphibians and reptiles?". Biology Open. 4 (7): 760–763. doi:10.1242/bio.012179. PMC 4571096. PMID 26015533.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Michaels, C.J.; Downie, J.R.; Campbell-Palmer, R. (2014). "The importance of enrichment for advancing amphibian welfare and conservation goals" (PDF). Amphibian & Reptile Conservation. 8 (1): 7–23. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-02-03.
  • ^ BBC news (2010). "Protest over 'cruel' frogs' legs chippy in Sunderland". BBC. Retrieved October 10, 2015.
  • ^ "Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986" (PDF). Home Office (UK). Retrieved September 23, 2015.
  • ^ "Animal Welfare Act 2006". UK Government. 2006. Retrieved September 25, 2015.
  • ^ Henriksen, S., Vaagland, H., Sundt-Hansen, L., May, R. and Fjellheim, A. (2003). "Consequences of pain perception in fish for catch and release, aquaculture and commercial fisheries" (PDF).{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ "Rules and regulations: Animal welfare act". New England Anti-Vivisection Society (NEAVS). Retrieved October 25, 2015.
  • Cognition

  • Animal consciousness
  • Animal language
  • Cognitive bias in animals
  • Cognitive ethology
  • Comparative cognition
  • Emotion in animals
  • Insect
  • Mirror test
  • Neuroethology
  • Observational learning
  • Primate archaeology
  • Tool use by non-humans
  • Vocal learning
  • Intelligence

  • talking
  • Cat
  • Cephalopod
  • Cetacean
  • Dog
  • Elephant
  • Fish
  • Primate
  • Swarm
  • Pain

  • Pain in animals
  • Pain in cephalopods
  • Pain in crustaceans
  • Pain in fish
  • Pain in invertebrates
  • Relation to brain

  • Brain-to-body mass ratio
  • Encephalization quotient
  • Neuroscience and intelligence
  • Number of neurons
  • Issues

  • Blood sports
  • Chick culling
  • Cormorant culling
  • Livestock dehorning
  • Eating live animals
  • Eating live seafood
  • Intensive animal farming
  • Intensive pig farming
  • Overview of discretionary invasive procedures on animals
  • Dairy farming
  • Poultry farming
  • Puppy mill
  • Vivisection
  • Welfare of farmed insects
  • Wild animal suffering
  • Wildlife farming
  • Feedback (pork industry)
  • Foam depopulation
  • Ventilation shutdown
  • Barn fire
  • Concepts

  • Animal psychopathology
  • Animal shelter
  • Animal welfare science
  • Anthrozoology
  • Behavioral enrichment
  • Compassionate conservation
  • Conservation welfare
  • Ethics of uncertain sentience
  • Ethical omnivorism
  • Five freedoms
  • Humane law enforcement
  • Intrinsic value in animal ethics
  • Rescue group
  • Three Rs principles
  • Welfare biology
  • Pain in
    nonhuman animals

  • Pain in amphibians
  • Pain in cephalopods
  • Pain in crustaceans
  • Pain in fish
  • Pain in invertebrates
  • Grimace scale
  • Organisations

  • Animal Defenders International
  • Animal Welfare Party
  • American Humane
  • Animal Aid Unlimited
  • Animal Welfare Board of India
  • Animal Welfare Institute
  • Badger Trust
  • Band of Mercy
  • Battersea
  • Blue Cross
  • Blue Cross of India
  • Cats Protection
  • Compassion in World Farming
  • CAWF
  • Cinnamon Trust
  • Dogs Trust
  • DSPCA
  • Eurogroup for Animals
  • Four Paws
  • Hedgehog Care
  • HSUS
  • International Animal Rescue
  • IFAW
  • International Society for Applied Ethology
  • ISPCA
  • List of animal welfare organizations
  • League Against Cruel Sports
  • Marine Connection
  • Network for Animals
  • Lord Dowding Fund for Humane Research
  • MSPCA-Angell
  • National Animal Welfare Trust
  • National Anti-Vivisection Society
  • OneKind
  • Party for Animal Welfare
  • People for Animals
  • People's Dispensary for Sick Animals
  • RNZSPCA
  • RSPCA
  • RSPCA Australia
  • Save Me
  • SSPCA
  • Tiggywinkles
  • UFAW
  • Wildlife Aid Foundation
  • World Animal Protection
  • World Horse Welfare
  • Publications

  • Our Dumb Animals
  • The Zoophilist
  • Activists
    and workers

    Contemporary

  • Cynthia Bathurst
  • Matthew E. Bershadker
  • Christa Blanke
  • Moira Brown
  • Jan Cameron
  • Judy MacArthur Clark
  • Kendra Coulter
  • Peter Ronald Davies
  • Petra Deimer
  • Alanna Devine
  • Robin Ganzert
  • Celia Hammond
  • Alison Hanlon
  • Brian Hare
  • Kathrin Glock
  • Emily Patterson-Kane
  • Philip Lymbery
  • Damien Mander
  • Christine Nicol
  • Wayne Pacelle
  • Sharon Pincott
  • Ilka Reinhardt
  • Jill Robinson
  • Naomi Sagara
  • Sepehr Salimi
  • Adolfo Sansolini
  • Rakesh Shukla
  • Karen Stockin
  • Patti Strand
  • Sally Varma
  • Tina M. Widowski
  • Historical

  • Emily Appleton
  • Madeleine Bemelmans
  • Henry Bergh
  • Mary Brancker
  • Victoria Braithwaite
  • Roger A. Caras
  • Jim Cronin
  • Brian Davies
  • Maria Dickin
  • Marjorie Doggett
  • Muriel Dowding
  • Alice Drakoules
  • Sarah J. Eddy
  • Aida McAnn Flemming
  • Lucy Furman
  • Julia Goddard
  • Robert Gower
  • Colesworthey Grant
  • George Greenwood
  • Peter Gurney
  • Velma Bronn Johnston
  • Catherine Victoria Hall
  • Rachel Hirschfeld
  • Sjoerd Hofstra
  • Albert Knapp
  • Titus Lander
  • Carla Lane
  • Etta Lemon
  • Victoria Lidiard
  • Terry Maple
  • Richard Martin
  • Shirley McGreal
  • Lady Mount Temple
  • Harriet Nevins
  • Catherine Louisa Pirkis
  • Eliza Phillips
  • Maude Gillette Phillips
  • Samuel Jackson Pratt
  • Wilfred Risdon
  • Peter Roberts
  • Jan Salter
  • Catherine Smithies
  • Thomas Bywater Smithies
  • Herbert Snow
  • Ellin Prince Speyer
  • Christine Stevens
  • Elisabeth Svendsen
  • Mary Tealby
  • Ralph Waldo Trine
  • Sidney Trist
  • Ethel Venton
  • Jessey Wade
  • Christopher Wathes
  • Franz Weber
  • Caroline Earle White
  • Emily Williamson
  • H. Fergie Woods
  • Alice Morgan Wright
  • Gretchen Wyler
  • Scholars
    and writers

    Contemporary

  • Jonathan Balcombe
  • Michael Balls
  • Geoffrey Barstow
  • Marc Bekoff
  • Donald Broom
  • Marian Dawkins
  • Bruce Fogle
  • Jane Goodall
  • Temple Grandin
  • Celia Haddon
  • Jane Hurst
  • Erich Hoyt
  • Barbara J. King
  • Alistair Lawrence
  • David Macdonald
  • Jeremy Marchant
  • Jeff McMahan
  • Daniel Mills
  • Sy Montgomery
  • Virginia Morell
  • Caitlin O'Connell-Rodwell
  • Clare Palmer
  • Susan J. Pearson
  • Dale Peterson
  • Jessica Pierce
  • Diana Reiss
  • Carl Safina
  • Matthew Scully
  • Paul Shapiro
  • Jennifer Skiff
  • Peter Wohlleben
  • Historical

  • Patrick Bateson
  • Beddow Bayly
  • Ernest Bell
  • Edward Berdoe
  • Arthur Broome
  • Jean Burden
  • Gerald Carson
  • Stephen Coleridge
  • Christian Adam Dann
  • J. Morewood Dowsett
  • Audrey Eyton
  • George Fleming
  • Ralph Fletcher
  • Ruth Harrison
  • C. W. Hume
  • E. Douglas Hume
  • James Macaulay
  • Basheer Ahmad Masri
  • Francis Orpen Morris
  • George Nicholson
  • Siobhan O'Sullivan
  • Charles Reinhardt
  • Francis H. Rowley
  • W. M. S. Russell
  • Chris Sherwin
  • Colin Spedding
  • William O. Stillman
  • Bell Taylor
  • Ernst von Weber
  • D. G. M. Wood-Gush
  • Frans de Waal
  • Hugh Wirth
  • William Youatt
  • Categories

  • Animal killing
  • Animal rights
  • Animal welfare and rights legislation
  • Animal sacrifice
  • Animal sanctuaries
  • Animal testing
  • Animal welfare and rights by country
  • Cruelty to animals
  • Dishes involving the consumption of live animals
  • Fur trade
  • People associated with animal welfare
  • Whaling
  • Religious
    considerations

  • Jhatka
  • Kutha
  • Shechita
  • Tza'ar ba'alei chayim
  • Rituals and
    festivals

  • Bous al carrer
  • Cock throwing
  • Combat de Reines
  • Eid al-Adha
  • Gadhimai festival
  • Goat throwing
  • Goose pulling
  • International Primate Day
  • Kapparot
  • Lychee and Dog Meat Festival
  • Monkey Buffet Festival
  • October Horse
  • Pushkar Camel Fair
  • Rapa das Bestas of Sabucedo
  • Running of the bulls
  • Surin Elephant Round-up
  • Toro embolado
  • World Animal Day
  • Legislation

  • Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 (UK)
  • Animal Welfare Act 1999 (NZ)
  • Animal Welfare Act of 1966 (US)
  • EU Directive 2010/63/EU (EU)
  • EU Directive 1999/74/EC (EU)
  • Horse Protection Act of 1970 (US)
  • Hunting Act 2004 (US)
  • List of international animal welfare conventions
  • Philippine Animal Welfare Act 1998 (Philippines)
  • Related

    Topics (overviews, concepts, issues, cases)

    Overviews

  • Animal rights by country or territory
  • Anarchism and animal rights
  • Animal rights and punk subculture
  • Animal cruelty–Holocaust analogies
  • Animal rights in Indian religions
  • Christianity and animal rights
  • History of animal rights
  • List of international animal welfare conventions
  • Moral status of animals in the ancient world
  • Timeline of animal welfare and rights
  • Total liberation
  • Universal Declaration on Animal Welfare
  • Concepts

  • Ahimsa
  • Animal cognition
  • Animal consciousness
  • Animal ethics
  • Animal–industrial complex
  • Animal law
  • Animal protectionism
  • Animal welfare
  • Animal-free agriculture
  • Anthrozoology
  • Argument from marginal cases
  • Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness
  • Carnism
  • Equal consideration of interests
  • Emotion in animals
  • Ethics of eating meat
  • Ethics of uncertain sentience
  • Ethology
  • Insects in ethics
  • Meat paradox
  • Nonviolence
  • Open rescue
  • Opposition to hunting
  • Personism
  • Replaceability argument
  • Sentiocentrism
  • Speciesism
  • Veganism
  • Vegaphobia
  • Vegetarianism
  • Issues

    Animal agriculture

  • Battery cage
  • Bile bear
  • Chick culling
  • Concentrated animal feeding operation
  • Fish farming
  • Fur farming
  • Fur trade
  • Insect farming
  • Intensive animal farming
  • Intensive pig farming
  • Livestock
  • Poultry farming
  • Slaughterhouse
  • Wildlife farming
  • Working animal
  • Feedback (pork industry)
  • Foam depopulation
  • Ventilation shutdown
  • Animal testing

  • Animal testing on non-human primates
  • Animal testing regulations
  • Labcorp Drug Development
  • Great ape research ban
  • Green Scare
  • Huntingdon Life Sciences
  • Model organism
  • Nafovanny
  • Operation Backfire
  • Vivisection
  • Animal welfare

  • Cruelty to animals
  • Pain in animals
  • Pain in amphibians
  • Pain in cephalopods
  • Pain in crustaceans
  • Pain in fish
  • Pain in invertebrates
  • Pain and suffering in laboratory animals
  • Welfare of farmed insects
  • Fishing

  • Fishing bait
  • Recreational fishing
  • Wild animals

  • Hare coursing
  • Hunting
  • International primate trade
  • Ivory trade
  • Predation problem
  • Seal hunting
  • Wild animal suffering
  • Wildlife management
  • Other

  • Animal sacrifice
  • Animal slaughter
  • Animal trial
  • Animals in sport
  • Live food
  • Live export
  • Cases

  • Cambridge University primates
  • McLibel case
  • Monkey selfie copyright dispute
  • Pit of despair
  • SHAC
  • Silver Spring monkeys
  • University of California, Riverside 1985 laboratory raid
  • Unnecessary Fuss
  • War of the currents
  • Methodologies

  • Hunt sabotage
  • Observances

  • World Day for the End of Speciesism
  • World Day for Laboratory Animals
  • World Day for the End of Fishing
  • Advocates (academics, writers, activists)

    Academics
    and writers

  • Tom Beauchamp
  • Marc Bekoff
  • Steven Best
  • Stephen St. C. Bostock
  • Paola Cavalieri
  • Stephen R. L. Clark
  • Alasdair Cochrane
  • J. M. Coetzee
  • Alice Crary
  • David DeGrazia
  • Daniel Dombrowski
  • Sue Donaldson
  • Josephine Donovan
  • Joan Dunayer
  • Mylan Engel
  • Catia Faria
  • Lawrence Finsen
  • Gary L. Francione
  • Robert Garner
  • Valéry Giroux
  • Lori Gruen
  • John Hadley
  • Oscar Horta
  • Dale Jamieson
  • Kyle Johannsen
  • Melanie Joy
  • Hilda Kean
  • Will Kymlicka
  • Renan Larue
  • Thomas Lepeltier
  • Andrew Linzey
  • Clair Linzey
  • Dan Lyons
  • David Nibert
  • Martha Nussbaum
  • Clare Palmer
  • Charles Patterson
  • David Pearce
  • Jessica Pierce
  • Evelyn Pluhar
  • Mark Rowlands
  • Richard D. Ryder
  • Steve F. Sapontzis
  • Jeff Sebo
  • Jérôme Segal
  • Peter Singer
  • Gary Steiner
  • Cass Sunstein
  • David Sztybel
  • Michael Tye
  • Tatjana Višak
  • Paul Waldau
  • Corey Lee Wrenn
  • Historical

  • David Renaud Boullier
  • Brigid Brophy
  • Peter Buchan
  • Mona Caird
  • Priscilla Cohn
  • Henry Crowe
  • Herman Daggett
  • Richard Dean
  • Wilhelm Dietler
  • William Hamilton Drummond
  • Edward Payson Evans
  • Thomas Ignatius Maria Forster
  • John Galsworthy
  • Thomas G. Gentry
  • Arthur Helps
  • John Hildrop
  • John Zephaniah Holwell
  • Soame Jenyns
  • Karl Christian Friedrich Krause
  • John Lawrence
  • Charles R. Magel
  • Jean Meslier
  • Mary Midgley
  • J. Howard Moore
  • José Ferrater Mora
  • Leonard Nelson
  • Edward Nicholson
  • Siobhan O'Sullivan
  • Rod Preece
  • Humphrey Primatt
  • James Rachels
  • Tom Regan
  • Nathaniel Peabody Rogers
  • Bernard Rollin
  • Henry Stephens Salt
  • Arthur Schopenhauer
  • Laurids Smith
  • John Styles
  • Thomas Tryon
  • Gary Varner
  • Johann Friedrich Ludwig Volckmann
  • Mary Anne Warren
  • Adam Gottlieb Weigen
  • Johann Heinrich Winckler
  • Steven M. Wise
  • Jon Wynne-Tyson
  • Activists

    Contemporary

  • Greg Avery
  • Matt Ball
  • Martin Balluch
  • Carole Baskin
  • Barbi Twins
  • Brigitte Bardot
  • Gene Baur
  • Yves Bonnardel
  • Joey Carbstrong
  • Aymeric Caron
  • Jake Conroy
  • Rod Coronado
  • Karen Dawn
  • Chris DeRose
  • John Feldmann
  • Bruce Friedrich
  • Juliet Gellatley
  • Tal Gilboa
  • Antoine Goetschel
  • Mark Gold
  • Brigitte Gothière
  • Wayne Hsiung
  • Charlotte Laws
  • Ronnie Lee
  • Howard Lyman
  • Evanna Lynch
  • Bill Maher
  • Keith Mann
  • Jim Mason
  • Dan Mathews
  • Jo-Anne McArthur
  • Luísa Mell
  • Virginia McKenna
  • Ingrid Newkirk
  • Heather Nicholson
  • Jack Norris
  • Ric O'Barry
  • David Olivier
  • Alex Pacheco
  • Craig Rosebraugh
  • Nathan Runkle
  • Jasmin Singer
  • Kim Stallwood
  • Lynda Stoner
  • Marianne Thieme
  • Darren Thurston
  • Christine Townend
  • Wendy Turner-Webster
  • Jerry Vlasak
  • Louise Wallis
  • Gary Yourofsky
  • That Vegan Teacher
  • Historical

  • Henry B. Amos
  • Bob Barker
  • Ernest Bell
  • Edith Carrington
  • Frances Power Cobbe
  • Joan Court
  • Karen Davis
  • Royal Dixon
  • Muriel Dowding
  • Elizabeth Farians
  • Emarel Freshel
  • André Géraud
  • Lewis Gompertz
  • James Granger
  • Nina Douglas-Hamilton
  • Barry Horne
  • Marie Huot
  • Lizzy Lind af Hageby
  • Jessie Mackay
  • Norm Phelps
  • Jill Phipps
  • Maud Ingersoll Probasco
  • Hans Ruesch
  • Nell Shipman
  • Henry Spira
  • Andrew Tyler
  • Gretchen Wyler
  • Movement (groups, parties)

    Groups

  • Animal Justice
  • Animal Justice Project
  • Animal Legal Defense Fund
  • Animal Liberation
  • Animal Liberation Front
  • Animal Rising
  • AnimaNaturalis
  • Anti-Vivisection Coalition
  • Anonymous for the Voiceless
  • Beauty Without Cruelty
  • Born Free Foundation
  • Centre for Animals and Social Justice
  • Chinese Animal Protection Network
  • Cruelty Free International
  • Direct Action Everywhere
  • Doctors Against Animal Experiments
  • Equanimal
  • Every Animal
  • Farm Animal Rights Movement
  • Faunalytics
  • Great Ape Project
  • Hunt Saboteurs Association
  • In Defense of Animals
  • Korea Animal Rights Advocates
  • L214
  • Last Chance for Animals
  • Massachusetts Animal Rights Coalition
  • Mercy for Animals
  • Oxford Centre for Animal Ethics
  • People for the Ethical Treatment of Animals
  • Rise for Animals
  • Sentience Politics
  • Uncaged Campaigns
  • United Activists for Animal Rights
  • United Poultry Concerns
  • UPF-Centre for Animal Ethics
  • Viva!
  • Voice for Animals Humane Society
  • Historical

  • Canadian Anti-Vivisection Society
  • Humanitarian League (1891–1919)
  • Oxford Group
  • Parties

  • Animal Politics EU (Europe)
  • Animal Protection Party of Canada (Canada)
  • Animal Justice Party of Finland (Finland)
  • Animals' Party (Sweden)
  • Animalist Movement (Italy)
  • Animalist Party Against Mistreatment of Animals (Spain)
  • DierAnimal (Belgium)
  • Human Environment Animal Protection (Germany)
  • Italian Animalist Party (Italy)
  • Party for the Animals (Netherlands)
  • People Animals Nature (Portugal)
  • V-Partei³ (Germany)
  • Activism

    Media (books, films, periodicals, albums)

    Books

  • Moral Inquiries on the Situation of Man and of Brutes (1824)
  • The Rights of Animals (1838)
  • The Ethics of Diet (1883)
  • Animals' Rights (1892)
  • Evolutional Ethics and Animal Psychology (1897)
  • The Universal Kinship (1906)
  • The New Ethics (1907)
  • Animals, Men and Morals (1971)
  • Animal Liberation (1975)
  • The Case for Animal Rights (1983)
  • Morals, Reason, and Animals (1987)
  • Zoos and Animal Rights (1993)
  • Animals, Property, and the Law (1995)
  • The Lives of Animals (1999)
  • Eternal Treblinka (2001)
  • Do Animals Have Rights? (2005)
  • Striking at the Roots (2008)
  • An American Trilogy (2009)
  • An Introduction to Animals and Political Theory (2010)
  • Animal Rights Without Liberation (2012)
  • Political Animals and Animal Politics (2014)
  • Animal (De)liberation (2016)
  • Sentientist Politics (2018)
  • Wild Animal Ethics (2020)
  • Animal Ethics in the Wild (2022)
  • Making a Stand for Animals (2022)
  • Films

  • A Cow at My Table (1998)
  • Shores of Silence (2000)
  • The Witness (2000)
  • Meet Your Meat (2002)
  • Legally Blonde 2 (2003)
  • The Meatrix (2003)
  • Peaceable Kingdom (2004)
  • Earthlings (2005)
  • Behind the Mask (2006)
  • Your Mommy Kills Animals (2007)
  • The Cove (2009)
  • Peaceable Kingdom: The Journey Home (2009)
  • Forks Over Knives (2011)
  • Vegucated (2011)
  • An Apology to Elephants (2013)
  • Speciesism: The Movie (2013)
  • The Ghosts in Our Machine (2013)
  • Unlocking the Cage (2016)
  • Dominion (2018)
  • Seaspiracy (2021)
  • Periodicals

    Journals

  • Between the Species
  • Cahiers antispécistes
  • Etica & Animali
  • Journal of Animal Ethics
  • Relations. Beyond Anthropocentrism
  • The Animals' Defender
  • Magazines

  • Bite Back
  • Muutoksen kevät
  • No Compromise
  • Satya
  • Albums

  • Tame Yourself (1991)
  • Manifesto (2008)
  • Salvation of Innocents (2014)
  • Onward to Freedom (2014)
  • Fairs and exhibitions


    Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Pain_in_amphibians&oldid=1234827432"

    Categories: 
    Pain in animals
    Amphibians
    Hidden categories: 
    CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list
    CS1 errors: missing periodical
    Articles with short description
    Short description matches Wikidata
     



    This page was last edited on 16 July 2024, at 10:03 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki