Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Background  





2 History of bonobos and chimpanzees  





3 Research  





4 See also  





5 References  














Panpanzee







Add links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Panpanzee
Specieschimpanzee
Sexfemale
Born1985 (1985)
DiedFebruary 9, 2014(2014-02-09) (aged 28–29)
Cause of deathdiabetes complications
Known forlearned 348 lexigram symbols and over 3,000 English words

Panpanzee the chimpanzee (1985 – February 9, 2014 at the age of 28–29 by diabetes complications), was born at the Language Research Center at Georgia State University in Atlanta, Georgia.[1] She lived the rest of her life at the Great Ape Trust in Iowa.[2] Her half-brother is Kanzi, a famous bonobo. Kanzi learned 348 lexigram symbols and over 3,000 words from the English language over her lifespan.[3]

Background[edit]

Kanzi and Panbanisha, two bonobos that Panpanzee grew up with at the Great Ape Trust

The influence of humans started for Panpanzee at a very young age of eight days old. She grew up with a bonobo named Panbanisha.[1] They were both in the same study, for five years, where the researchers taught them lexigram symbols and words from the English language. Kanzi was also getting research done to him at the Language Research Center during this time. He had already known 256 lexigram symbols by the time Panpanzee and Panbanisha had started their research.[3] This research was conducted by Sue Savage-Rumbaugh and her research team. Savage-Rumbaugh is a primatologist and an experimental psychologist, where she studies the upbringings of non-human primates and their capability to learn, understand, and speak a language. Most of her work today is on bonobos. This research on bonobos has made her known for “one of the world’s leading ape language researchers.” [4]

History of bonobos and chimpanzees[edit]

A part of this study was to see if there was a difference between bonobos and chimpanzees since they are very similar species. Both species shared a common ancestor after humans split from the chimpanzees and bonobos between four and seven million years ago in Africa. The split between the common ancestors of the chimpanzee and bonobos then took place about 1.5 to 2 million years ago with the two lineages giving rise to the two current extant species.

In the past, bonobos were incorrectly relegated to subspecies status within the species chimpanzee. It is now understood that bonobos are an entirely different species. After Pan paniscus was differentiated and named as a new species to science, "bonobo" developed into its accepted common name. As bonobos aren't chimpanzees, old common names containing that word like "bonobo chimpanzee" or "pygmy chimpanzee" are generally considered outdated misnomers.

Bonobos are more sociable, more sexual, and not as aggressive as chimpanzees. The Bonobos are the only members of the great ape family (which includes humans) that do not regularly kill their own kind. [5]

Research[edit]

One goal of Panpanzees and Panbanishas research was to see the effects of human enculturation on the chimps. Another goal was for the chimpanzee and bonobo to be able to comprehend spoken and symbolic language. To reach this goal the teachers had keyboards with lexigram on them. They had spoken communication to go with the lexigram in order for Panpanzee and Panbanisha to learn both.[1]

During the study, Panpanzee hardly utilized any of the symbol communication. She relied on gesturing at a young age. Her gestural communication was her main source of communication. For Panpanzee to show the researchers what she understood she would lead someone by a hand or guide them until they understood what she was saying. While Panpanzee was still using gestural communications, Panbanisha had moved on to symbolic communications. Panbanisha also could fully comprehend speech by the time she was around two to two and a half years old. When Panpanzee was around two years old her vocabulary started to increase and her use of lexigrams went up. Panpanzee took longer to pick up vocabulary words than bonobos did. She did not learn fifty vocabulary words until she was thirty-four months old. By this time, a typical child has already learned up to several hundred vocabulary words.[6] Before Panpanzee began to progress, the researchers almost removed her from the study because of the lack of improvement. She was not picking up words fast enough and not learning enough symbols. The researchers assumed this was because chimpanzees did not have the capacity to be able to learn a language. They also assumed it was bonobos genetic superiority that led to Kanzi to have the success that he did. However, they stuck with her and a few months later Panpanzee began to show some understanding of a few words in the English language.[7] Even though Panpanzee started to show a lot of progress the researchers still removed her from the study when she was five years old.[1]

By the time she became a grown chimpanzee, Panpanzee had learned one hundred and twenty-eight different lexigrams while being able to comprehend one hundred and fifty English words. This made Panpanzees understanding of English more precise than her understanding of lexigrams, which is also the same for Kanzi.[7] She could now point out lexigrams when someone states a word that it corresponds with that particular lexigram symbol.[5]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c d "Panzee obituary" (PDF). Retrieved November 18, 2018.
  • ^ "Chimpanzee | Greatapetrust.org". www.greatapetrust.org. Retrieved 2018-11-20.
  • ^ a b Raffaele, Paul. "Speaking Bonobo". Smithsonian. Retrieved 2018-11-20.
  • ^ "Dr. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh". The MY HERO Project. Retrieved 2018-11-20.
  • ^ a b "Bonobos Join Chimps as Closest Human Relatives". Science | AAAS. 2012-06-13. Retrieved 2018-11-20.
  • ^ Brakke, Karen (1996). "The Development of Language Skills in Production" (PDF). Language and Communications. 16 (4): 361–380. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.41.8816. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(96)00018-3. S2CID 15212109. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-11-21.
  • ^ a b Hillix, William A.; Rumbaugh, Duane M. (2004). "A Cultural Approach to Language Learning". Animal Bodies, Human Minds: Ape, Dolphin, and Parrot Language Skills. Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects. Springer. pp. 151–187. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-4512-2_10. ISBN 978-1-4419-3400-0.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Panpanzee&oldid=1193905538"

    Category: 
    Individual chimpanzees
    Hidden categories: 
    Articles with short description
    Short description matches Wikidata
    Articles with 'species' microformats
     



    This page was last edited on 6 January 2024, at 05:50 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki