Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 History  





2 Conditions for occurrence  





3 Applications  



3.1  Sound reinforcement systems  





3.2  Ambience extraction  





3.3  Multichannel audio decoding  





3.4  Haas kicker  







4 See also  





5 References  





6 Further reading  














Precedence effect






Deutsch
Français

Italiano

Русский

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 




In other projects  



Wikimedia Commons
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


The precedence effectorlaw of the first wavefront is a binaural psychoacoustical effect concerning sound reflection and the perceptionofechoes. When two versions of the same sound presented are separated by a sufficiently short time delay (below the listener's echo threshold), listeners perceive a single auditory event; its perceived spatial location is dominated by the location of the first-arriving sound (the first wave front). The lagging sound does also affect the perceived location; however, its effect is mostly suppressed by the first-arriving sound.

The Haas effect was described in 1949 by Helmut Haas in his Ph.D. thesis.[1] The term "Haas effect" is often loosely taken to include the precedence effect which underlies it.

History

[edit]

Joseph Henry published "On The Limit of Perceptibility of a Direct and Reflected Sound" in 1851.[2]

The "law of the first wavefront" was described and named in 1948 by Lothar Cremer.[3]

The "precedence effect" was described and named in 1949 by Wallach et al.[4] They showed that when two identical sounds are presented in close succession they will be heard as a single fused sound. In their experiments, fusion occurred when the lag between the two sounds was in the range 1 to 5 ms for clicks, and up to 40 ms for more complex sounds such as speech or piano music. When the lag was longer, the second sound was heard as an echo.

Additionally, Wallach et al. demonstrated that when successive sounds coming from sources at different locations were heard as fused, the apparent location of the perceived sound was dominated by the location of the sound that reached the ears first (i.e. the first-arriving wavefront). The second-arriving sound had only a very small (albeit measurable) effect on the perceived location of the fused sound. They designated this phenomenon as the precedence effect, and noted that it explains why sound localization is possible in the typical situation where sounds reverberate from walls, furniture and the like, thus providing multiple, successive stimuli. They also noted that the precedence effect is an important factor in the perception of stereophonic sound.

Wallach et al. did not systematically vary the intensities of the two sounds, although they cited research by Langmuir et al.[5] which suggested that if the second-arriving sound is at least 15 dB louder than the first, the precedence effect breaks down.

The "Haas effect" derives from a 1951 paper by Helmut Haas.[6] In 1951 Haas examined how the perception of speech is affected in the presence of a single, coherent sound reflection.[7] To create anechoic conditions, the experiment was carried out on the rooftop of a freestanding building. Another test was carried out in a room with a reverberation time of 1.6 s. The test signal (recorded speech) was emitted from two similar loudspeakers at locations 45° to the left and to the right in 3 m distance to the listener.

Haas found that humans localize sound sources in the direction of the first arriving sound despite the presence of a single reflection from a different direction, and that in such cases only a single auditory event is perceived. A reflection arriving later than 1 ms after the direct sound increases the perceived level and spaciousness (more precisely the perceived width of the sound source). A single reflection arriving at a delay of between 5 and 30 ms can be up to 10 dB louder than the direct sound without being perceived as a secondary auditory event (i.e. it does not sound like an echo). This time span varies with the reflection level. If the direct sound is coming from the same direction the listener is facing, the reflection's direction has no significant effect on the results. If the reflection's higher frequencies are attenuated, echo suppression continues to occur even if the delay between the sounds is somewhat longer. Increased room reverberation time also expands the time span available for echo suppression.[8]

Conditions for occurrence

[edit]

The precedence effect occurs if the subsequent wave fronts arrive between 2 ms and about 50 ms later than the first wave front. This range is signal dependent. For speech, the precedence effect disappears for delays above 50 ms, but for music, the precedence effect can still occur with delays approaching 100 ms.[9]

In two-click lead–lag experiments, localization effects include aspects of summing localization, localization dominance, and lag discrimination suppression. The last two are generally considered to be aspects of the precedence effect:[10]

For time delays above 50 ms (for speech) or some 100 ms (for music) the delayed sound is perceived as an echo of the first-arriving sound, and each sound direction is localized separately and correctly. The time delay for perceiving echoes depends on the signal characteristics. For signals with impulse characteristics, echoes are perceived for delays above 50 ms. For signals with a nearly constant amplitude, the threshold before perceiving an echo can be enhanced up to time differences of 1 to 2 seconds.

A special appearance of the precedence effect is the Haas effect. Haas showed that the precedence effect appears even if the level of the delayed sound is up to 10 dB higher than the level of the first wave front. In this case, the precedence effect only works for delays between 10 and 30 ms.

Applications

[edit]

The precedence effect is important for hearing in enclosed spaces. With the help of this effect, it remains possible to determine the direction of a sound source (e.g. the direction of a speaker) even in the presence of wall reflections.

Sound reinforcement systems

[edit]

Haas's findings can be applied to sound reinforcement systems and public address systems. The signal for loudspeakers placed at distant locations from a stage may be delayed electronically by an amount equal to the time sound takes to travel through the air from the stage to the distant location, plus about 10 to 20 milliseconds, and played at a level up to 10 dB louder than the sound reaching this location directly from the stage. The first arrival of sound from the source on stage determines perceived localization whereas the slightly later sound from delayed loudspeakers simply increases the perceived sound level without negatively affecting localization. In this configuration, the listener will localize all sound from the direction of the direct sound, but they will benefit from the higher sound level, which has been enhanced by the loudspeakers.[11]

Ambience extraction

[edit]

The precedence effect can be employed to increase the perception of ambience during the playback of stereo recordings.[12] If two more speakers are placed to the left and right of the listener (in addition to the two main speakers), and are fed with the same program material but delayed by 10 to 20 milliseconds, the random-phase ambience components of the sound will become sufficiently decorrelated that they cannot be localized. This effectively extracts the recording's existing ambience, while leaving its foreground "direct" sounds still appearing to come from the front.[13][14]

Multichannel audio decoding

[edit]

The effect was taken into account and exploited in the psychoacoustics of the Fosgate Tate 101A SQ decoder, developed by Jim Fosgate in consultation with Peter Scheiber and Martin Willcocks, to produce much better spatiality and directionality in matrix decoding of 4-2-4 (SQ quadraphonic) audio.

Haas kicker

[edit]

Many older LEDE ("live end, dead end") control room designs featured so-called "Haas kickers" – reflective panels placed at the rear to create specular reflections which were thought to provide a wider stereo listening area or raise intelligibility.[15] However, what is beneficial for one type of sound is detrimental to others, so Haas kickers, like compression ceilings, are no longer commonly found in control rooms.[16]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ "Pro Audio Reference". Retrieved 2020-04-18. After Helmut Haas's doctorate dissertation presented to the University of Gottingen, Gottingen, Germany as『Über den Einfluss eines Einfachechos auf die Hörsamkeit von Sprache;』translated into English by Dr. Ing. K.P.R. Ehrenberg, Building Research Station, Watford, Herts., England Library Communication no. 363, December, 1949; reproduced in the United States as "The Influence of a Single Echo on the Audibility of Speech," J. Audio Eng. Soc., Vol. 20 (Mar. 1972), pp. 145-159.
  • ^ Proceedings American Association Advancement of Science vol v, pp 42, 48, May 6, 1851. Reprinted in "The Scientific Writings of Joseph Henry Vol 1," Smithsonian Institution, 1886, pp 296-296
  • ^ Cremer, L. (1948): "Die wissenschaftlichen Grundlagen der Raumakustik", Bd. 1. Hirzel-Verlag Stuttgart.
  • ^ Wallach, H., Newman, E. B., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1949). "The precedence effect in sound localization," The American Journal of Psychology, 62, 315–336.
  • ^ Langmuir, I., Schaefer, V. J., Ferguson, C. V., & Hennelly, E. F. (1944). "A study of binaural perception of the direction of a sound source," OSRD Report 4079, PB number 31014, Office of Technical Services, U. S. Department of Commerce.
  • ^ Haas, H. (1951). "Uber den Einfluss eines Einfachechos auf die Horsamkeit von Sprache," Acustica, 1, 49–58.
  • ^ Arthur H. Benade (1990). Fundamentals of musical acoustics. Courier Dover Publications. p. 204. ISBN 978-0-486-26484-4.
  • ^ Haas, H. "The Influence of a Single Echo on the Audibility of Speech", JAES Volume 20 Issue 2 pp. 146-159; March 1972
  • ^ Blauert, J.: Spatial hearing - the psychophysics of human sound localization; MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts (1983), chapter 3.1
  • ^ Litovsky, R.Y.; Colburn, H.S.; Yost, W.A.; Guzman, S.J. (1999). "The precedence effect" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 106 (4 Pt 1): 1633–16. Bibcode:1999ASAJ..106.1633L. doi:10.1121/1.427914. PMID 10530009.
  • ^ Audio, NTi. "How To Setup for a Live Sound Event" (PDF). www.nti-audio.com.
  • ^ Madsen, E. Roerbaek (October 1970). "Extraction of Ambiance Information from Ordinary Recordings". Journal of the Audio Engineering Society. 18 (5): 490–496.
  • ^ Katz, Bob (March 1988). "Extraction vs Generation". Stereophile.
  • ^ Katz, Bob (2007). Mastering Audio: The Art and the Science. Taylor & Francis. pp. 229–237. ISBN 978-0240808376.
  • ^ Davis, Patronis "Sound System Engineering", Focal Press; 3 edition (September 20, 2006)
  • ^ Philip Newell "Recording Studio Design", Focal Press; 2 edition (December 22, 2007)
  • Further reading

    [edit]
    Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Precedence_effect&oldid=1225116663"

    Categories: 
    Acoustics
    Hearing
    Audio engineering
    Sound
    Perception
    Psychoacoustics
    Hidden categories: 
    Articles with short description
    Short description matches Wikidata
     



    This page was last edited on 22 May 2024, at 13:25 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki