Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Detailed definition  





2 Prevalence and training  





3 See also  





4 References  














Relative pitch






Čeština
Deutsch
Español
Français
עברית
Nederlands

Norsk bokmål
Oʻzbekcha / ўзбекча
Polski
Русский
Slovenščina
Svenska

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Relative pitch is the ability of a person to identify or re-create a given musical note by comparing it to a reference note and identifying the interval between those two notes. For example, if the notes Do and Fa are played on a piano, a person with relative pitch would be able to identify the second note from the first note given that they know that the first note is Do without looking.

Detailed definition[edit]

Relative pitch implies some or all of the following abilities:[1]

This last criterion, which applies not only to singers but also to instrumentalists who rely on their own skill to determine the precise pitch of the notes played (wind instruments, fretless string instruments like violin or viola, etc.), is an essential skill for musicians in order to play successfully with others. An example, is the different concert pitches used by orchestras playing music from different styles (a baroque orchestra using period instruments might decide to use a higher-tuned pitch).

Compound intervals (intervals greater than an octave) can be more difficult to detect than simple intervals (intervals less than an octave).[citation needed]

Interval recognition is used to identify chords, and can be applied to accurately tune an instrument with respect to a given reference tone, even when the tone is not in concert pitch.[citation needed]

Prevalence and training[edit]

Unlike absolute pitch (sometimes called "perfect pitch"), relative pitch is quite common among musicians, especially musicians who are used to playing "by ear", and a precise relative pitch is a constant characteristic among good musicians.

Unlike perfect pitch, relative pitch can be developed through ear training.[2] Computer-aided ear training is becoming a popular tool for musicians and music students, and various software is available for improving relative pitch.[citation needed]

Some music teachers teach their students relative pitch by having them associate each possible interval with the first two notes of a popular song. Another method of developing relative pitch is playing melodies by ear on a musical instrument, especially one that, unlike a piano or other keyboard or fretted instrument, requires a specific manual or blown adjustment for each particular tone.

Indian musicians learn relative pitch by singing intervals over a drone, which Mathieu (1997) described in terms of occidental just intonation terminology.[3] [page needed] Many Western ear training classes used solfège to teach students relative pitch, while others use numerical sight-singing.

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Leipold, Simon; Greber, Marielle; Sele, Silvano; Jäncke, Lutz (15 October 2019). "Neural patterns reveal single-trial information on absolute pitch and relative pitch perception" (PDF). NeuroImage. 200: 132–141. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.030. PMID 31238164.
  • ^ Humphries, Lee (2008). Learning to Sight-Sing: The mental mechanics of aural imagery (PDF). Vol. 1. Minneapolis, MN: Thinking Applied.
  • ^ Mathieu, W.A. (1997). Harmonic Experience: Tonal harmony from its natural origins to its modern expression. Inner Traditions. ISBN 0-89281-560-4.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Relative_pitch&oldid=1194775912"

    Categories: 
    Music cognition
    Music psychology
    Pitch (music)
    Cognitive musicology
    Singing
    Hidden categories: 
    Articles needing additional references from August 2018
    All articles needing additional references
    All articles with unsourced statements
    Articles with unsourced statements from August 2018
    Wikipedia articles needing page number citations from August 2018
     



    This page was last edited on 10 January 2024, at 17:57 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki