Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Similar effects  





2 Evidence of effects on behavior  



2.1  Effects on pro-social behavior  



2.1.1  Pro-social experiments  







2.2  Donation experiment  





2.3  More on studies  







3 See also  





4 References  














Watching-eye effect






العربية
Oʻzbekcha / ўзбекча
 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


A sticker in German warning that the reader is being "video monitored". Even just the presence of an eye symbol on a sticker can be enough to change a person's behavior.

The watching-eye effect says that people behave more altruistically and exhibit less antisocial behavior in the presence of images that depict eyes, because these images insinuate that they are being watched. Eyes are strong signals of perception for humans. They signify that our actions are being seen and paid attention to even through mere depictions of eyes.[1]

It has been demonstrated that these effects are so pronounced that even depictions of eyes are enough to trigger them. This means that people need not actually be watched, but that a simple photograph of eyes is enough to elicit feelings that individuals are being watched which can impact their behavior to be more pro-social and less antisocial.[2] Empirical psychological research has continually shown that the visible presence of images depicting eyes nudges people towards slightly, but measurably more honest and more pro-social behavior.[3]

The concept is part of the psychology of surveillance and has implications for the areas of crime reduction and prevention without increasing actual surveillance, just by psychological measures alone. By simply inserting signs depicting eyes and leading others to believe they are being watched, crime can be reduced, as it leads to behavior that is more socially acceptable.[4][additional citation(s) needed]

Similar effects[edit]

The effect differs from the psychic staring effect in that the latter describes the feeling of being watched, whereas individuals who succumb to the watching-eye effect usually affect our behaviour through the subconscious level.[2]

Evidence of effects on behavior[edit]

Effects on pro-social behavior[edit]

There is evidence that images of eyes being present cause people to behave pro-socially. Pro-social behavior is acting in a way or with the intent that benefits others.[5] There are two forms of motivation that support this. One being negative motivation that makes people want to avoid behavior that is wrong and violates the norm. They want to keep up a positive social image, or be seen improving their image rather than worsening it.[citation needed] The second being positive motivation to get a reward or benefits in the future. They believed that under watching eyes that if they behaved in a positive manner that benefited others, they were likely to get paid back for it in the future.[3]

Pro-social experiments[edit]

Certain studies have shown that under the influence of eyes people will behave as truthfully honest. Under controlled groups without images of eyes present people were more likely to behave anti-socially and lie for the benefit of others.[3] People lean toward honesty rather than acting generously to keep a good image in these situations in order to avoid violating norms. In these situations honesty is often chosen since it is seen as the most pro-social behavior.[3]

There are more examples of studies that show that pro-social behavior is more likely under watchful behavior. People were more likely to share things such as money in games that had to do with economics when presented with images with eyes.[3] People were also shown to be more likely to pick up trash at bus stops and pick up after themselves in a cafeteria,[3] they were less likely to commit bicycle theft,[4] and people were much more likely to give the full amount of money for their coffee on certain days that images of eyes were put up nearby.[3]

In an experiment on littering funded by the School of Psychology at Newcastle University it was found that places that already had trash on the ground tending to have an increase in littering, showing that people tend to behave in ways that seem socially acceptable. Likewise, it was discovered that images of eyes that insinuated watching caused a reduce in littering however, the reduction of littering was mainly only present when there were also larger groups of people around. The findings of this study added to the idea that watching eyes reduce anti-social behavior and increase people to behave more pro-socially.[6]

Donation experiment[edit]

In situations where the image of eyes were present people were also more likely to be generous with donations and give more. One study testing this was done at the University of Virginia by Caroline Kelsey.[7] The study was done at a children's museum where there was a donation box at the front desk. Data was collected from this setting for 28 weeks, testing more than 34,100 people who visited in the span of this time. Each week the sign over the box that usually read "Donations would be appreciated" changed to primarily images of eyes or other inanimate objects such as chairs or noses with some wording with it. Throughout each week the number of people who visited the museum was recording along with the total amount of donations made. By the end of the study it was found that patrons donated more under the presence of eyes on the signs rather than other inanimate objects.[8]

More on studies[edit]

Other studies in relation to the watching-eye-effect show that people are more cooperative and aware of themselves when their identity is exposed as opposed to when they are acting anonymously. They act more respectfully and appropriately because their reputation is at risk when they are being watched by others or feel that they are being watched.[citation needed][9] Even in some studies that insisted to their participants that their actions were anonymous they were still more generous because they felt identified by the eyes.[1]

Some studies argue that it may not be the effect of these eyes that gives people incentive to be more generous, but the number of people that are around them that make them feel peer pressure to conform to more pro-social behavior.[10]

See also[edit]

  • Evil eye
  • Eye contact
  • Fake security camera
  • Gaze
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Security theater
  • Situation awareness
  • Subject-expectancy effect
  • References[edit]

    1. ^ a b Mackenzie, Debora. "Big Brother' eyes make us act more honestly". New Scientist. Retrieved 2020-03-20.
  • ^ a b Dear, Keith; Dutton, Kevin; Fox, Elaine (2019-05-01). "Do 'watching eyes' influence antisocial behavior? A systematic review & meta-analysis". Evolution and Human Behavior. 40 (3): 269–280. Bibcode:2019EHumB..40..269D. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.01.006. ISSN 1090-5138.
  • ^ a b c d e f g Oda, Ryo; Kato, Yuta; Hiraishi, Kai (2015-09-01). "The Watching-Eye Effect on Prosocial Lying". Evolutionary Psychology. 13 (3): 1474704915594959. doi:10.1177/1474704915594959. ISSN 1474-7049. PMC 10480987. PMID 37924187.
  • ^ a b Nettle, Daniel; Nott, Kenneth; Bateson, Melissa (2012-12-12). "'Cycle Thieves, We Are Watching You': Impact of a Simple Signage Intervention against Bicycle Theft". PLOS ONE. 7 (12): e51738. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...751738N. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051738. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3520908. PMID 23251615.
  • ^ "Prosocial Behavior". Ethics Unwrapped. Retrieved 2020-11-10.
  • ^ Bateson, Melissa; Callow, Luke; Holmes, Jessica R.; Roche, Maximilian L. Redmond; Nettle, Daniel (2013-12-05). "Do Images of 'Watching Eyes' Induce Behaviour That Is More Pro-Social or More Normative? A Field Experiment on Littering". PLOS ONE. 8 (12): e82055. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...882055B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082055. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3855385. PMID 24339990.
  • ^ Kelsey, Caroline; Vaish, Amrisha; Grossmann, Tobias (2018-12-01). "Eyes, More Than Other Facial Features, Enhance Real-World Donation Behavior". Human Nature. 29 (4): 390–401. doi:10.1007/s12110-018-9327-1. ISSN 1936-4776. PMID 30324537. S2CID 53108021.
  • ^ "People donate more when they sense they are being watched: Study suggests that eyes serve as cues to monitor and elicit charitable behavior". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2020-03-20.
  • ^ Neilands, Patrick, Rebecca Hassall, Frederique Derks, Amalia PM Bastos, and Alex H. Taylor (2020). "Watching eyes do not stop dogs stealing food: evidence against a general risk-aversion hypothesis for the watching-eye effect". Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 1153. Bibcode:2020NatSR..10.1153N. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-58210-4. PMC 6981177. PMID 31980699.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • ^ Oda, Ryo (2019-05-28). "Is the watching-eye effect a fluke?". Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science. 10 (1): 4–6. doi:10.5178/lebs.2019.68. ISSN 1884-927X.

  • Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Watching-eye_effect&oldid=1223651119"

    Categories: 
    Psychological effects
    Cognition
    Cognitive biases
    Cognitive psychology
    Hidden categories: 
    CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list
    Articles with short description
    Short description is different from Wikidata
    All articles needing additional references
    Articles needing additional references from August 2022
    All articles with unsourced statements
    Articles with unsourced statements from March 2020
     



    This page was last edited on 13 May 2024, at 14:05 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki