Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Overview  





2 North American English  



2.1  Resistance  





2.2  Origin  







3 England  





4 Scotland  





5 See also  





6 Notes  





7 References  





8 Bibliography  





9 External links  














Cotcaught merger: Difference between revisions






Español
Simple English

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 




Print/export  



















Appearance
   

 





Help
 

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Browse history interactively
 Previous editNext edit 
Content deleted Content added
Tyrui (talk | contribs)
308 edits
→‎Overview: I've added Western New England.
m Spelling/grammar/punctuation/typographical correction - italic tag
Line 124: Line 124:

In North America, the first evidence of the merger (or its initial conditions) comes from western Pennsylvania as far back as the data show.<ref>Johnson, D. E., Durian, D., & Hickey, R. (2017). New England. Listening to the Past: Audio Records of Accents of English, 234.</ref> From there, it entered [[Upper Canada]] (what is now [[Ontario]]). In the mid-19th century, the merger also independently began in eastern New England,<ref name="Johnson"/> possibly influencing the [[Canadian Maritimes]], though the merger is in evidence as early as the 1830s in both regions of Canada: Ontario and the Maritimes.<ref name="Dollinger"/> Fifty years later, the merger "was already more established in Canada" than in its two U.S. places of origin.<ref name="Dollinger">{{cite journal |url = https://www.academia.edu/4006014 |title = Written sources of Canadian English: phonetic reconstruction and the low-back vowel merger |first = Stefan |last = Dollinger |website = Academia.edu |date = 2010 |access-date = 2016-03-19}}</ref> In Canadian English, further westward spread was completed more quickly than in English of the United States.

In North America, the first evidence of the merger (or its initial conditions) comes from western Pennsylvania as far back as the data show.<ref>Johnson, D. E., Durian, D., & Hickey, R. (2017). New England. Listening to the Past: Audio Records of Accents of English, 234.</ref> From there, it entered [[Upper Canada]] (what is now [[Ontario]]). In the mid-19th century, the merger also independently began in eastern New England,<ref name="Johnson"/> possibly influencing the [[Canadian Maritimes]], though the merger is in evidence as early as the 1830s in both regions of Canada: Ontario and the Maritimes.<ref name="Dollinger"/> Fifty years later, the merger "was already more established in Canada" than in its two U.S. places of origin.<ref name="Dollinger">{{cite journal |url = https://www.academia.edu/4006014 |title = Written sources of Canadian English: phonetic reconstruction and the low-back vowel merger |first = Stefan |last = Dollinger |website = Academia.edu |date = 2010 |access-date = 2016-03-19}}</ref> In Canadian English, further westward spread was completed more quickly than in English of the United States.



Two traditional theories of the merger's origins have been longstanding in linguistics: one group of scholars argues for an independent North American development, while others argue for contact-induced language change via [[Scots-Irish Americans|Scots-Irish]] or Scottish immigrants to North America. In fact, both theories may be true but for different regions. The merger's appearance in western Pennsylvania is better explained as an effect of Scots-Irish settlement,<ref>Evanini, Keelan (2009). "[https://repository.upenn.edu/dissertations/AAI3405374 The permeability of dialect boundaries: A case study of the region surrounding Erie, Pennsylvania]". University of Pennsylvania; dissertations available from ProQuest''. AAI3405374. pp. 254-255.</ref> but in eastern New England,<ref name="Johnson">Johnson, Daniel Ezra (2010). "LOW VOWELS OF NEW ENGLAND: HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT". Publication of the American Dialect Society 95 (1): 13–41. doi: https://doi.org/10.1215/-95-1-13. p. 40.</ref> and perhaps the American West,<ref>Grama, James; Kennedy, Robert (2019). "2. Dimensions of Variance and Contrast in the Low Back Merger and the Low-Back-Merger Shift". The Publication of the American Dialect Society. 104, p. 47.</ref> as an internal structural development. Canadian linguist [[Charles Boberg]] considers the issue unresolved.<ref>{{Cite book|title = The English language in Canada|last = Boberg|first = Charles|publisher = Cambridge|year = 2010|location = Cambridge|pages = 199?}}</ref> A third theory has been used to explain the merger's appearance specifically in northeastern Pennsylvania: an influx of Polish- and other Slavic-language speakers whose [[English language learner|learner English]] failed to maintain the distinction.<ref>Herold, Ruth. (1990). "Mechanisms of merger: The implementation and distribution of the low back merger in eastern Pennsylvania". Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.</ref>

Two traditional theories of the merger's origins have been longstanding in linguistics: one group of scholars argues for an independent North American development, while others argue for contact-induced language change via [[Scots-Irish Americans|Scots-Irish]] or Scottish immigrants to North America. In fact, both theories may be true but for different regions. The merger's appearance in western Pennsylvania is better explained as an effect of Scots-Irish settlement,<ref>Evanini, Keelan (2009). "[https://repository.upenn.edu/dissertations/AAI3405374 The permeability of dialect boundaries: A case study of the region surrounding Erie, Pennsylvania]". University of Pennsylvania; dissertations available from ProQuest''. AAI3405374. pp. 254-255.''</ref> but in eastern New England,<ref name="Johnson">Johnson, Daniel Ezra (2010). "LOW VOWELS OF NEW ENGLAND: HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT". Publication of the American Dialect Society 95 (1): 13–41. doi: https://doi.org/10.1215/-95-1-13. p. 40.</ref> and perhaps the American West,<ref>Grama, James; Kennedy, Robert (2019). "2. Dimensions of Variance and Contrast in the Low Back Merger and the Low-Back-Merger Shift". The Publication of the American Dialect Society. 104, p. 47.</ref> as an internal structural development. Canadian linguist [[Charles Boberg]] considers the issue unresolved.<ref>{{Cite book|title = The English language in Canada|last = Boberg|first = Charles|publisher = Cambridge|year = 2010|location = Cambridge|pages = 199?}}</ref> A third theory has been used to explain the merger's appearance specifically in northeastern Pennsylvania: an influx of Polish- and other Slavic-language speakers whose [[English language learner|learner English]] failed to maintain the distinction.<ref>Herold, Ruth. (1990). "Mechanisms of merger: The implementation and distribution of the low back merger in eastern Pennsylvania". Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.</ref>



==England==

==England==


Revision as of 02:34, 11 May 2023

[kʰäʔ] for cot and [kʰɔʔ] for caught

[kʰɑt] for cot and caught alike

The cotcaught merger, also known as the LOT–THOUGHT mergerorlow back merger, is a sound change present in some dialects of English where speakers do not distinguish the vowel phonemes in words like cot versus caught. Cot and caught (along with bot and bought, pond and pawned, etc.) is an example of a minimal pair that is lost as a result of this sound change. The phonemes involved in the cot–caught merger, the low back vowels, are typically represented in the International Phonetic Alphabetas/ɒ/ and /ɔ/, respectively (or, in North America, co-occurring with the father–bother merger, as /ɑ/ and /ɔ/). The merger is typical of most Canadian and Scottish English dialects as well as some Irish and U.S. English dialects.

An additional vowel merger, the father–bother merger, which spread through North America in the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries, has resulted today in a three-way merger in which most Canadian and many U.S. accents have no vowel difference in words like PALM /ɑ/, LOT /ɒ/, and THOUGHT /ɔ/. However, /ɔr/ as in NORTH participates in a separate phenomenon in most North American English: the NORTH–FORCE merger, in which this vowel before /r/ can be phonemicized as the GOAT vowel,[1] transcribed variously thus as /or/[2]or/oʊr/.[3]

Overview

The shift causes the vowel sound in words like cot, nod and stock and the vowel sound in words like caught, gnawed and stalktomerge into a single phoneme; therefore the pairs cot and caught, stock and stalk, nod and gnawed become perfect homophones, and shock and talk, for example, become perfect rhymes. The cot–caught merger is completed in the following dialects:

Examples of homophonous pairs
/ɑ/or/ɒ/ (written a, o, ol) /ɔ/ (written au, aw, al, ough) IPA (using ⟨ɒ⟩ for the merged vowel[a])
bobble bauble ˈbɒbəl
body bawdy ˈbɒdi
bot bought ˈbɒt
box balks ˈbɒks
chock chalk ˈtʃɒk
clod clawed ˈklɒd
cock caulk ˈkɒk
cod cawed ˈkɒd
collar caller ˈkɒlə(r)
cot caught ˈkɒt
don dawn ˈdɒn
fond fawned ˈfɒnd
hock hawk ˈhɒk
holler hauler ˈhɒlə(r)
hottie haughty ˈhɒti
knot nought ˈnɒt
knotty naughty ˈnɒti
nod gnawed ˈnɒd
not nought ˈnɒt
odd awed ˈɒd
pod pawed ˈpɒd
pond pawned ˈpɒnd
rot wrought ˈrɒt
sod sawed ˈsɒd
sot sought ˈsɒt
stock stalk ˈstɒk
tot taught ˈtɒt
wok walk ˈwɒk

North American English

On this map of English-speaking North America, the green dots represent speakers who have completely merged the vowels of cot and caught. The dark blue dots represent speakers who have completely resisted the merger. The medium blue dots represent speakers with a partial merger (either production or perception but not both), and the yellow dots represent speakers with the merger in transition. Based on the work of Labov, Ash and Boberg.[13]

Nowhere is the shift more complex than in North American English. The presence of the merger and its absence are both found in many different regions of the North American continent, where it has been studied in greatest depth, and in both urban and rural environments. The symbols traditionally used to transcribe the vowels in the words cot and caught as spoken in American English are ⟨ɑ⟩ and ⟨ɔ⟩, respectively, although their precise phonetic values may vary, as does the phonetic value of the merged vowel in the regions where the merger occurs.

Even without taking into account the mobility of the American population, the distribution of the merger is still complex; there are pockets of speakers with the merger in areas that lack it, and vice versa. There are areas where the merger has only partially occurred, or is in a state of transition. For example, based on research directed by William Labov (using telephone surveys) in the 1990s, younger speakers in Kansas, Nebraska, and the Dakotas exhibited the merger while speakers older than 40 typically did not.[14][15] The 2003 Harvard Dialect Survey, in which subjects did not necessarily grow up in the place they identified as the source of their dialect features, indicates that there are speakers of both merging and contrast-preserving accents throughout the country, though the basic isoglosses are almost identical to those revealed by Labov's 1996 telephone survey. Both surveys indicate that, as of the 1990s, approximately 60% of American English speakers preserved the contrast, while approximately 40% merged the phonemes. Further complicating matters are speakers who merge the phonemes in some contexts but not others, or merge them when the words are spoken unstressed or casually but not when they're stressed.

Speakers with the merger in northeastern New England still maintain a phonemic distinction between a fronted and unrounded /ɑ/ (phonetically [ä]) and a back and usually rounded /ɔ/ (phonetically [ɒ]), because in northeastern New England (unlike in Canada and the Western United States), the cot–caught merger occurred without the father–bother merger. Thus, although northeastern New Englanders pronounce both cot and caughtas[kɒt], they pronounce cartas[kät].

Labov et al. also reveal that, for about 15% of respondents, a specific /ɑ//ɔ/ merger before /n/ but not before /t/ (or other consonants) is in effect, so that Don and dawn are homophonous, but cot and caught are not. In this case, a distinct vowel shift (which overlaps with the cot–caught merger for all speakers who have indeed completed the cot–caught merger) is taking place, identified as the Don–dawn merger.[16]

Resistance

According to Labov, Ash, and Boberg,[17] the merger in North America is most strongly resisted in three regions:

In the three American regions above, sociolinguists have studied three phonetic shifts that can explain their resistance to the merger. The first is the fronting of /ɑ/ found in the Inland North; speakers advance the LOT vowel /ɑ/ as far as the cardinal [a] (the open front unrounded vowel), thus allowing the THOUGHT vowel /ɔ/ to lower into the phonetic environment of [ɑ] without any merger taking place.[18] The second situation is the raising of the THOUGHT vowel /ɔ/ found in the New York City, Philadelphia and Baltimore accents, in which the vowel is raised and diphthongized to [ɔə⁓oə], or, less commonly, [ʊə], thus keeping that vowel notably distinct from the LOT vowel /ɑ/.[18] The third situation occurs in the South, in which vowel breaking results in /ɔ/ being pronounced as upgliding [ɒʊ], keeping it distinct from /ɑ/.[18] None of these three phonetic shifts, however, is certain to preserve the contrast for all speakers in these regions. Some speakers in all three regions, particularly younger ones, are beginning to exhibit the merger despite the fact that each region's phonetics should theoretically block it.[citation needed]

African American Vernacular English accents have traditionally resisted the cot–caught merger, with LOT pronounced [ɑ̈] and THOUGHT traditionally pronounced [ɒɔ], though now often [ɒ~ɔə]. Early 2000s research has shown that this resistance may continue to be reinforced by the fronting of LOT, linked through a chain shift of vowels to the raising of the TRAP, DRESS, and perhaps KIT vowels. This chain shift is called the "African American Shift".[19] However, there is still evidence of AAVE speakers picking up the cot–caught merger in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania,[20]inCharleston, South Carolina,[21] Florida and Georgia,[22] and in parts of California.[22]

Origin

In North America, the first evidence of the merger (or its initial conditions) comes from western Pennsylvania as far back as the data show.[23] From there, it entered Upper Canada (what is now Ontario). In the mid-19th century, the merger also independently began in eastern New England,[24] possibly influencing the Canadian Maritimes, though the merger is in evidence as early as the 1830s in both regions of Canada: Ontario and the Maritimes.[25] Fifty years later, the merger "was already more established in Canada" than in its two U.S. places of origin.[25] In Canadian English, further westward spread was completed more quickly than in English of the United States.

Two traditional theories of the merger's origins have been longstanding in linguistics: one group of scholars argues for an independent North American development, while others argue for contact-induced language change via Scots-Irish or Scottish immigrants to North America. In fact, both theories may be true but for different regions. The merger's appearance in western Pennsylvania is better explained as an effect of Scots-Irish settlement,[26] but in eastern New England,[24] and perhaps the American West,[27] as an internal structural development. Canadian linguist Charles Boberg considers the issue unresolved.[28] A third theory has been used to explain the merger's appearance specifically in northeastern Pennsylvania: an influx of Polish- and other Slavic-language speakers whose learner English failed to maintain the distinction.[29]

England

In London's Cockney accent, a cot–caught merger is possible only in rapid speech. The THOUGHT vowel has two phonemically distinct variants: closer /oː/ (phonetically [ ~ ~ ɔo]) and more open /ɔə/ (phonetically [ɔə ~ ɔwə ~ ɔː]). The more open variant is sometimes neutralized in rapid speech with the LOT vowel /ɒ/ (phonetically [ɒ ~ ɔ]) in utterances such as [sˈfɔðɛn] (phonemically /ɑɪ wəz ˈfɔə ðen/) for I was four then. Otherwise /ɔə/ is still readily distinguished from /ɒ/ by length.[30]

Scotland

Outside North America, another dialect featuring the merger is Scottish English. Like in New England English, the cot–caught merger occurred without the father–bother merger. Therefore, speakers still retain the distinction between /a/ and /ɔ/.

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Except for pairs that only apply to accents that also have the father–bother merger, for which /ɑ/ is used.

References

  1. ^ Wells (1982), p. 479.
  • ^ Kenyon, John S.; Thomas A. Knott (1949) [1943]. A Pronouncing Dictionary of American English. Springfield, Mass.: G. & C. Merriam. ISBN 0-87779-047-7.
  • ^ "ore". Dictionary.com Unabridged (Online). 2023.
  • ^ a b Wells 1982, p. ?
  • ^ Heggarty, Paul; et al., eds. (2013). "Accents of English from Around the World". University of Edinburgh. Retrieved 2016-12-12.
  • ^ Wells 1982, p. 438
  • ^ a b c Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), pp. 60–1
  • ^ Gagnon, C. L. (1999). Language attitudes in Pittsburgh: 'Pittsburghese' vs. standard English. Master's thesis. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh.
  • ^ Dubois, Sylvia; Horvath, Barbara (2004). "Cajun Vernacular English: phonology". In Kortmann, Bernd; Schneider, Edgar W. (eds.). A Handbook of Varieties of English: A Multimedia Reference Tool. New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 409–10.
  • ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. 218
  • ^ Wells 1982, p. 626
  • ^ "Singapore English" (PDF). Videoweb.nie.edu.sg. Retrieved 2016-12-12.
  • ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. 122
  • ^ Gordon (2005)
  • ^ "Map 1". Ling.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2016-12-12.
  • ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. 217
  • ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), pp. 56–65
  • ^ a b c Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), chpt. 11
  • ^ Thomas, Erik R. (September 2007). "Phonological and Phonetic Characteristics of African American Vernacular English: Phonological and Phonetic Characteristics of AAVE". Language and Linguistics Compass. 1 (5): 450–475. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00029.x.
  • ^ Eberhardt (2008).
  • ^ Baranowski (2013).
  • ^ a b Jones (2020), p. 165.
  • ^ Johnson, D. E., Durian, D., & Hickey, R. (2017). New England. Listening to the Past: Audio Records of Accents of English, 234.
  • ^ a b Johnson, Daniel Ezra (2010). "LOW VOWELS OF NEW ENGLAND: HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT". Publication of the American Dialect Society 95 (1): 13–41. doi: https://doi.org/10.1215/-95-1-13. p. 40.
  • ^ a b Dollinger, Stefan (2010). "Written sources of Canadian English: phonetic reconstruction and the low-back vowel merger". Academia.edu. Retrieved 2016-03-19.
  • ^ Evanini, Keelan (2009). "The permeability of dialect boundaries: A case study of the region surrounding Erie, Pennsylvania". University of Pennsylvania; dissertations available from ProQuest. AAI3405374. pp. 254-255.
  • ^ Grama, James; Kennedy, Robert (2019). "2. Dimensions of Variance and Contrast in the Low Back Merger and the Low-Back-Merger Shift". The Publication of the American Dialect Society. 104, p. 47.
  • ^ Boberg, Charles (2010). The English language in Canada. Cambridge: Cambridge. pp. 199?.
  • ^ Herold, Ruth. (1990). "Mechanisms of merger: The implementation and distribution of the low back merger in eastern Pennsylvania". Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.
  • ^ Wells 1982, pp. 305, 310, 318–319
  • Bibliography

    External links


    Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Cot–caught_merger&oldid=1154227050"

    Categories: 
    Dialects of English
    Splits and mergers in English phonology
    Hidden categories: 
    Pages using the Phonos extension
    Pages with plain IPA
    Use American English from March 2019
    All Wikipedia articles written in American English
    Articles with short description
    Short description is different from Wikidata
    Articles with hAudio microformats
    All articles with unsourced statements
    Articles with unsourced statements from April 2023
    Articles with unsourced statements from March 2021
    Articles to be expanded from December 2016
    All articles to be expanded
    Articles using small message boxes
     



    This page was last edited on 11 May 2023, at 02:34 (UTC).

    This version of the page has been revised. Besides normal editing, the reason for revision may have been that this version contains factual inaccuracies, vandalism, or material not compatible with the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki