Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  

































Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 History  





2 Explanation  





3 Auditory analogue  





4 See also  





5 References  



5.1  Notes  







6 External links  














Barberpole illusion






العربية
Deutsch
Español
Français
Bahasa Indonesia

Svenska
Tiếng Vit

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 




In other projects  



Wikimedia Commons
 
















Appearance
   

 






From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


An example of the barberpole illusion. The grating is actually drifting downwards and to the right at 45 degrees, but its motion is captured by the elongated axis of the aperture.

The barberpole illusion is a visual illusion that reveals biases in the processing of visual motion in the human brain. This visual illusion occurs when a diagonally striped pole is rotated around its vertical axis (horizontally), it appears as though the stripes are moving in the direction of its vertical axis (downwards in the case of the animation to the right)[1] rather than around it.[2]

History[edit]

The barber's pole is commonly found outside barber shops.

In 1929, psychologist J.P. Guilford informally noted a paradox in the perceived motion of stripes on a rotating barber pole. The barber pole turns in place on its vertical axis, but the stripes appear to move upwards rather than turning with the pole.[3] Guilford tentatively attributed the phenomenon to eye movements, but acknowledged the absence of data on the question.

In 1935, Hans Wallach published a comprehensive series of experiments related to this topic,[4] but since the article was in German it was not immediately known to English-speaking researchers. An English summary of the research was published in 1976[5] and a complete English translation of the 1935 paper was published in 1996.[6] Wallach's analysis focused on the interaction between the terminal points of the diagonal lines and the implicit aperture created by the edges of the pole.

Explanation[edit]

In this example the motion of the grating is identical to that in example 1, but the aperture is isotropic.

This illusion occurs because a bar or contour within a frame of reference provides ambiguous information about its "real" direction of movement. The actual motion of the line has many possibilities. The shape of the aperture thus tends to determine the perceived direction of motion for an otherwise identically moving contour. A vertically elongated aperture makes vertical motion dominant whereas a horizontally elongated aperture makes horizontal motion dominant. In the case of a circular or square aperture, the perceived direction of movement is usually orthogonal to the orientation of the stripes (diagonal, in this case). The perceived direction of movement relates to the termination of the line's end points within the inside border of the occluder. The vertical aperture, for instance, has longer edges at the vertical orientation, creating a larger number of terminators unambiguously moving vertically. This stronger motion signal forces us to perceive vertical motion. Functionally, this mechanism has evolved to ensure that we perceive a moving pattern as a rigid surface moving in one direction.[7]

Individual motion-sensitive neurons in the visual system have only limited information, as they see only a small portion of the visual field (a situation referred to as the "aperture problem"). In the absence of additional information the visual system prefers the slowest possible motion: i.e., motion orthogonal to the moving line.[8] The neurons which may correspond to perceiving barber-pole-like patterns have been identified in the visual cortexofferrets.[9]

Auditory analogue[edit]

A similar effect occurs in the Shepard's tone, which is an auditory illusion.[1][2]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

Notes[edit]

  1. ^ a b "Barber Pole Illusion". sandlotscience.com. Archived from the original on 28 November 2010. Retrieved November 14, 2010.
  • ^ a b Massaro, Dominic W., ed. (Spring 2007). "Book Reviews: What Are Musical Paradox and Illusion?" (PDF). American Journal of Psychology. 120 (1). University of California, Santa Cruz: 123–170, 124, 132.
  • ^ Guilford, J.P. (1929) "Illusory Movement from a Rotating Barber Pole." American Journal of Psychology 41: 686–687.
  • ^ Wallach, H. (1935). "Ueber visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung". Psychologische Forschung. 20: 325–380. doi:10.1007/bf02409790. S2CID 145400184.
  • ^ Wallach, H. (1976). On perception. Quadrangle/New York Times Book Company. chap. IX.1
  • ^ Wuerger, S., Shapley, R., & Rubin, N. (1996). "'On the visually perceived direction of motion' by Hans Wallach: 60 years later." Perception-London, 25: 1317–1368.
  • ^ Todorović, Dejan (2002). "A new variant of the barberpole effect: Psycholphysical data and computer simulations" (PDF). Psihologija. 35 (3–4). Serbia, Yugoslavia: Laboratory for Experimental Psychology, University of Belgrade: 209–223 UDC 159.937.075. doi:10.2298/psi0203209t. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 23, 2011. Retrieved November 26, 2010.
  • ^ Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-31967-9
  • ^ Lees, Kevin (July 15, 2003). "Rethinking How the Brain Sees Visual Features: Duke neurobiologists study brain's visual-processing region". Duke News. Durham, North Carolina: Duke University. Archived from the original on July 26, 2010. Retrieved December 14, 2010.
  • External links[edit]


    Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Barberpole_illusion&oldid=1103097746"

    Category: 
    Optical illusions
    Hidden category: 
    Commons category link is on Wikidata
     



    This page was last edited on 8 August 2022, at 09:26 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki