Jump to content
 







Main menu
   


Navigation  



Main page
Contents
Current events
Random article
About Wikipedia
Contact us
Donate
 




Contribute  



Help
Learn to edit
Community portal
Recent changes
Upload file
 








Search  



























Create account

Log in
 









Create account
 Log in
 




Pages for logged out editors learn more  



Contributions
Talk
 



















Contents

   



(Top)
 


1 Early life  





2 Career  





3 Personal life  





4 Research  



4.1  Bee perception  





4.2  Dances as language  



4.2.1  Round dance  





4.2.2  Waggle dance  





4.2.3  "Dialects"  







4.3  Other work  







5 Honors and decorations  





6 Publications  



6.1  In German  





6.2  In English  







7 Notes  





8 References  





9 External links  














Karl von Frisch






العربية
تۆرکجه
 / Bân-lâm-gú
Беларуская
Български
Català
Čeština
Deutsch
Eesti
Ελληνικά
Español
Esperanto
Euskara
فارسی
Français
Gaeilge
Galego

Հայերեն
Hrvatski
Ido
Bahasa Indonesia
Italiano
עברית

Қазақша
Kiswahili
Latina
Latviešu
Magyar

مصرى
مازِرونی
Bahasa Melayu
Nederlands

Norsk bokmål
Norsk nynorsk
Occitan
Oʻzbekcha / ўзбекча
پنجابی
Polski
Português
Română
Русский
Simple English
Slovenščina
کوردی
Suomi
Svenska
Татарча / tatarça
Türkçe
Українська
اردو
Tiếng Vit

Yorùbá

 

Edit links
 









Article
Talk
 

















Read
Edit
View history
 








Tools
   


Actions  



Read
Edit
View history
 




General  



What links here
Related changes
Upload file
Special pages
Permanent link
Page information
Cite this page
Get shortened URL
Download QR code
Wikidata item
 




Print/export  



Download as PDF
Printable version
 




In other projects  



Wikimedia Commons
 


















From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
 


Karl Ritter von Frisch
In traditional dress, with his honey bees
Born(1886-11-20)20 November 1886
Died12 June 1982(1982-06-12) (aged 95)
EducationUniversity of Vienna (PhD, 1910)
University of Munich
Known forBehaviour and perception of bees
Spouse(s)Margarete, née Mohr
Parents
  • Marie Exner (mother)
  • Awards
  • Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine (1973)
  • Balzan Prize for Biology (1962)
  • Kalinga Prize (1958)
  • Magellanic Premium (1956)
  • Lieben Prize (1921)
  • Scientific career
    FieldsEthology
    InstitutionsRostock University, Breslau University, University of Munich, University of Graz
    Notable studentsIngeborg Beling, Maximilian Renner

    Karl Ritter[a] von Frisch, ForMemRS[1] (20 November 1886 – 12 June 1982) was a German-Austrian ethologist who received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1973, along with Nikolaas Tinbergen and Konrad Lorenz.[2][3]

    His work centered on investigations of the sensory perceptions of the honey bee and he was one of the first to translate the meaning of the waggle dance. His theory, described in his 1927 book Aus dem Leben der Bienen (translated into English as The Dancing Bees), was disputed by other scientists and greeted with skepticism at the time. Only much later was it shown to be an accurate theoretical analysis.[4]

    Early life[edit]

    Karl Ritter von Frisch was the son of the surgeon and urologist Anton von Frisch (1849–1917), by his marriage to Marie Exner. Karl was the youngest of four sons, all of whom became university professors. Von Frisch was of partial Jewish heritage.[5] His maternal grandmother was of Jewish origin.[6]

    Karl studied at the University of Vienna under Hans Leo Przibram and in Munich under Richard von Hertwig, initially in the field of medicine, but later turned to the natural sciences. He received his doctorate in 1910 and in the same year started work as an assistant in the zoology department of the University of Munich.

    Career[edit]

    In 1912, he became a lecturer in zoology and comparative anatomy there; and in 1919 was promoted to a professorship. His research on honeybees was continued by his student Ingeborg Beling. In 1921 he went to Rostock University as a professor of zoology and director of an institute. In 1923 he accepted the offer of a chair at Breslau University, returning in 1925 to Munich University, where he became the head of the institute of zoology.

    Von Frisch attracted negative attention from the Nazi regime, among other things[7] for employing Jewish assistants, including many women, and for practicing "Jewish science". Eventually Frisch was forced into retirement, but the decision was reversed because of his research on nosema infections in bees.[8]

    The institute of zoology was destroyed in the Second World War, and in 1946, Frisch went to work at the University of Graz, remaining there until 1950, when he returned to the reopened Munich institute. He retired in 1958 but continued his research.

    Personal life[edit]

    Karl Ritter von Frisch married Margarete, née Mohr, who died in 1964. Their son, Otto Ritter von Frisch, was director of the Brunswick natural history museum between 1977 and 1995.

    Research[edit]

    Frisch studied aspects of animal behaviour, including animal navigation, in the Carniolan honey bee[citation needed] (Apis mellifera carnica), a subspecies of the European honey bee.

    Carniolan honey bee on a goldenrod flower head

    Bee perception[edit]

    Frisch discovered that bees can distinguish various blossoming plants by their scent, and that each bee is "flower constant".[9] Surprisingly, their sensitivity to a "sweet" taste is only slightly stronger than in humans. He thought it possible that a bee's spatial sense of smell arises from the firm coupling of its olfactory sense with its tactile sense. Frisch was the second to demonstrate that honey bees had color vision, the first being Charles Henry Turner which he accomplished by using classical conditioning.[10] He trained bees to feed on a dish of sugar water set on a colored card. He then set the colored card in the middle of a set of gray-toned cards. If the bees see the colored card as a shade of gray, then they will confuse the blue card with at least one of the gray-toned cards; bees arriving to feed will visit more than one card in the array. On the other hand, if they have color vision, then the bees visit only the blue card, as it is visually distinct from the other cards.[10] A bee's color perception is comparable to that of humans, but with a shift away from the red toward the ultraviolet part of the spectrum. For that reason bees cannot distinguish red from black (colorless), but they can distinguish the colors white, yellow, blue and violet. Color pigments which reflect UV radiation expand the spectrum of colors which can be differentiated. For example, several blossoms which may appear to humans to be of the same yellow color will appear to bees as having different colors (multicolored patterns) because of their different proportions of ultraviolet.

    Frisch's investigation of a bee's powers of orientation were significant. He discovered that bees can recognize the desired compass direction in three different ways: by the Sun, by the polarization pattern of the blue sky, and by the Earth's magnetic field, whereby the Sun is used as the main compass, with the alternatives reserved for the conditions arising under cloudy skies or within a dark beehive.[11]

    Light scattered in a blue sky forms a characteristic pattern of partially polarized light which is dependent on the position of the Sun and invisible to human eyes. With a UV receptor in each of the lens units of a compound eye, and a UV filter oriented differently in each of these units, a bee is able to detect this polarization pattern. A small piece of blue sky is enough for a bee to recognize the pattern changes occurring over the course of a day. This provides not only directional but also temporal information.

    Frisch proved that variations in the position of the Sun over the course of a day provided bees with an orientation tool. They use this capability to obtain information about the progression of the day deep inside a dark beehive comparable to what is known from the position of the Sun. This makes it possible for the bees to convey always up-to-date directional information during their waggle dance, without having to make a comparison with the Sun during long dance phases. This provides them not only with alternative directional information, but also with additional temporal information.

    Bees have an internal clock with three different synchronization or timekeeping mechanisms. If a bee knows the direction to a feeding place found during a morning excursion, it can also find the same location, as well as the precise time at which this source provides food, in the afternoon, based on the position of the sun.[12]

    Based on the magnetic field, the alignment of the plane of a honeycomb under construction (e.g., the new honeycomb of a swarm) will be the same as that of the home hive of the swarm, according to Frisch. By experiment, even deformed combs bent into a circle can be produced.

    The vertical alignment of the honeycomb is attributed by Frisch to the ability of bees to identify what is vertical with the help of their head used as a pendulum together with a ring of sensory cells in the neck.

    Dances as language[edit]

    Knowledge about feeding places can be relayed from bee to bee. The means of communication is a special dance of which there are two forms:

    Round dance[edit]

    The "round dance" provides the information that there is a feeding place in the vicinity of the beehive at a distance between 50 and 100 meters, without the particular direction being given. By means of close contact among the bees it also supplies information about the type of food (blossom scent).

    The foraging bee ... begins to perform a kind of "round dance". On the part of the comb where she is sitting she starts whirling around in a narrow circle, constantly changing her direction, turning now right, now left, dancing clockwise and anti-clockwise, in quick succession, describing between one and two circles in each direction. This dance is performed among the thickest bustle of the hive. What makes it so particularly striking and attractive is the way it infects the surrounding bees; those sitting next to the dancer start tripping after her, always trying to keep their outstretched feelers on close contact with the tip of her abdomen. ... They take part in each of her manoeuvrings so that the dancer herself, in her mad wheeling movements, appears to carry behind her a perpetual comet's tail of bees.[13]

    Waggle dance[edit]

    The waggle dance
    Interpretation of the waggle dance: direction relative to the sun is shown by angle to the vertical; distance by the time taken on the central stretch.

    The "waggle dance" is used to relay information about more distant food sources. In order to do this, the dancing bee moves forward a certain distance on the vertically hanging honeycomb in the hive, then traces a half circle to return to her starting point, whereupon the dance begins again. On the straight stretch, the bee "waggles" with her posterior. The direction of the straight stretch contains the information about the direction of the food source, the angle between the straight stretch and the vertical being precisely the angle which the direction of flight has to the position of the sun. The distance to the food source is relayed by the time taken to traverse the straight stretch, one second indicating a distance of approximately one kilometer (so the speed of the dance is inversely related to the actual distance). The other bees take in the information by keeping in close contact with the dancing bee and reconstructing its movements. They also receive information via their sense of smell about what is to be found at the food source (type of food, pollen, propolis, water) as well as its specific characteristics. The orientation functions so well that the bees can find a food source with the help of the waggle dance even if there are hindrances they must detour around like an intervening mountain.

    As to a sense of hearing, Frisch could not identify this perceptive faculty, but it was assumed that vibrations could be sensed and used for communication during the waggle dance. Confirmation was later provided by Dr. Jürgen Tautz, a bee researcher at Würzburg University's Biocenter.[14]

    "Dialects"[edit]

    The linguistic findings described above were based on Frisch's work primarily with the Carnica variety of bees. Investigations with other varieties led to the discovery that language elements were variety-specific, so that how distance and direction information is relayed varies greatly.[citation needed]

    Other work[edit]

    Frisch's honey bee work included the study of the pheromones that are emitted by the queen bee and her daughters, which maintain the hive's very complex social order. Outside the hive, the pheromones cause the male bees, or drones, to become attracted to a queen and mate with her. Inside the hive, the drones are not affected by the odor.[15]

    Honors and decorations[edit]

    Publications[edit]

    In German[edit]

    In English[edit]

    Notes[edit]

    1. ^ Regarding personal names: Ritter is a title, translated approximately as Sir (denoting a Knight), not a first or middle name. There is no equivalent female form.

    References[edit]

  • ^ Michelsen, A. (2003). "Karl von Frisch lecture. Signals and flexibility in the dance communication of honeybees". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 189 (3): 165–174. doi:10.1007/s00359-003-0398-y. PMID 12664092. S2CID 7241637.
  • ^ Raju, T. N. (1999). "The Nobel chronicles. 1973: Karl von Frisch (1886-1982); Konrad Lorenz (1903-89); and Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907-88)". Lancet. 354 (9184): 1130. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(05)76931-2. PMID 10509540. S2CID 54236399.
  • ^ Riley, J.; Greggers, U.; Smith, A.; Reynolds, D.; Menzel, R. (2005). "The flight paths of honeybees recruited by the waggle dance". Nature. 435 (7039): 205–207. Bibcode:2005Natur.435..205R. doi:10.1038/nature03526. PMID 15889092. S2CID 4413962.
  • ^ Raffles, Sina Najafi and Hugh. "The Language of the Bees: An Interview with Hugh Raffles | Sina Najafi and Hugh Raffles". cabinetmagazine.org. Retrieved 2020-10-07.
  • ^ "Charlotte Exner". 3 October 1814.
  • ^ Frisch, Karl von (1957). Erinnerungun eines Biologen (Memories of a Biologist. Berlin: Springer. [page needed]
  • ^ Deichmann, Ute (1992). Biologists under Hitler: Expulsion, Careers, Research. Frankfurt am Main, New York: Harvard University Press. pp. 40–48. ISBN 978-0674074040.
  • ^ Frisch (1962), pp. 45–51.
  • ^ a b Backhaus, W. (1993). "Color vision and color choice behavior of the honey bee". Apidologie. 24 (3): 309–331. doi:10.1051/apido:19930310. Karl von Frisch (1914) was the first to demonstrate in behavioral experiments of this kind that bees possess a true color sense. He demonstrated that honeybees are able to distinguish a blue-colored card-board from a series of cardboards which appeared grey to the human eye.
  • ^ Frisch (1962), pp. 93–96.
  • ^ Frisch (1962), pp. 137–147.
  • ^ Frisch (1962), p. 102 ff.
  • ^ Rohrseitz, K.; Tautz, J. (1999). "Honey bee dance communication: Waggle run direction coded in antennal contacts?". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 184 (4): 463–470. doi:10.1007/s003590050346. S2CID 38520988.
  • ^ Frisch, K. von; Rösch, G. A. (1926).『Neue Versuche über die Bedeutung von Duftorgan und Pollenduft für die Verständigung im Bienenvolk』[New experiments on the importance of the scent organ and the smell of pollen for understanding of the bee colony]. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie (in German). 4: 1–21. doi:10.1007/BF00341784. S2CID 2685182.
  • ^ "Book of Members, 1780–2010: Chapter F" (PDF). American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved 15 April 2011.
  • ^ "K. von Frisch (1886–1982)". Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved 21 July 2015.
  • External links[edit]


    Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Karl_von_Frisch&oldid=1179854836"

    Categories: 
    1886 births
    1982 deaths
    Scientists from Vienna
    Austrian beekeepers
    Ethologists
    University of Vienna alumni
    Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich alumni
    Foreign Members of the Royal Society
    Academic staff of the Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich
    Academic staff of the University of Rostock
    Academic staff of the University of Breslau
    Academic staff of the University of Graz
    Fellows of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences
    Members of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences
    Foreign associates of the National Academy of Sciences
    Nobel laureates in Physiology or Medicine
    Austrian Nobel laureates
    Nobel laureates from Austria-Hungary
    Austrian people of Czech-Jewish descent
    Kalinga Prize recipients
    Recipients of the Pour le Mérite (civil class)
    Recipients of the Austrian Decoration for Science and Art
    Grand Crosses with Star and Sash of the Order of Merit of the Federal Republic of Germany
    20th-century German zoologists
    Members of the Royal Swedish Academy of Sciences
    Hidden categories: 
    Wikipedia articles needing page number citations from January 2018
    CS1 German-language sources (de)
    Articles with short description
    Short description is different from Wikidata
    Pages using infobox person with multiple spouses
    Articles with hCards
    Pages using Template:Post-nominals with customized linking
    All articles with unsourced statements
    Articles with unsourced statements from November 2018
    Articles with unsourced statements from October 2019
    Nobelprize template using Wikidata property P8024
    Articles with Internet Archive links
    Articles with FAST identifiers
    Articles with ISNI identifiers
    Articles with VIAF identifiers
    Articles with WorldCat Entities identifiers
    Articles with BIBSYS identifiers
    Articles with BNC identifiers
    Articles with BNE identifiers
    Articles with BNF identifiers
    Articles with BNFdata identifiers
    Articles with CANTICN identifiers
    Articles with GND identifiers
    Articles with ICCU identifiers
    Articles with J9U identifiers
    Articles with KBR identifiers
    Articles with LCCN identifiers
    Articles with Libris identifiers
    Articles with LNB identifiers
    Articles with NDL identifiers
    Articles with NKC identifiers
    Articles with NLA identifiers
    Articles with NLG identifiers
    Articles with NTA identifiers
    Articles with PLWABN identifiers
    Articles with VcBA identifiers
    Articles with CINII identifiers
    Articles with Leopoldina identifiers
    Articles with DTBIO identifiers
    Articles with Trove identifiers
    Articles with SNAC-ID identifiers
    Articles with SUDOC identifiers
     



    This page was last edited on 12 October 2023, at 21:50 (UTC).

    Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.



    Privacy policy

    About Wikipedia

    Disclaimers

    Contact Wikipedia

    Code of Conduct

    Developers

    Statistics

    Cookie statement

    Mobile view



    Wikimedia Foundation
    Powered by MediaWiki